Systematic Literature Review on Mindset and the Benefits in Living
New Normal Life
Ira Adelina
a
, Vida Handayani
b
and Maria Yuni Megarini
c
Faculty of Psychology, Maranatha Christian University, Surya Sumantri 65, Bandung, Indonesia
Keywords: Stress, Mindset, COVID-19, Pandemic, New Normal.
Abstract: Pandemics are associated with lots of psychosocial stressors, such of separation of family and friends,
shortages of food and medicine, wage loss, social isolation, financial hardship, death, trauma, and so on. The
psychological effects of the pandemic will likely be more pronounced, more widespread, and longer lasting
than the purely somatic effects of infection. This pandemic period causes intense stress for individuals.
Mindset, as a belief whether ability and intelligence are fixed or changeable traits, plays a critical role in how
we cope in life’s challenges. This research uses descriptive method, in a form of systematic literature review
from more than 50 articles, taken from psychological and medical journals in the last 35 years. The journals
related to the pandemic situation from medical and psychological perspectives, along with its interventions.
Based on this review, we conclude that mindset plays an important role in individual’s appraisals and
responses to stressors. Responses given by individual’s can be adaptive responses that lead to effective coping,
or maladaptive and lead to coping that is ineffective and even malfunctioning and disrupted health during
pandemic.
1 INTRODUCTION
Pandemics are large-scale epidemics afflicting
millions of people across multiple countries,
sometimes spreading throughout the globe (WHO,
2010b). According to Killbourne (1977) in Taylor
(2019), for a virus or bacterium to cause a pandemic
it must be an organism for which most people do not
have pre-existing immunity, transmitting easily from
person to person, and causing severe illness. Diseases
causing pandemics are part of a group of conditions
known as emerging infectious diseases, which
include newly identified pathogens as well as re-
emerging ones.
Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19), which
reported in an outbreak in 2019 in Wuhan, Hubei
province, China, is caused by the SARS-CoV-2
virus. Coronavirus (CoV) is among the main
pathogenic organisms that affect the respiratory
system in humans. In December 2019, the prevalence
of the virus increased at an epidemic rate since its first
occurrence in Wuhan. On 11 February 2020, the
a
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3720-8211
b
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3544-9559
c
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6563-6251
novel virus began to cause pneumonia, and was
named as coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) by
the World Health Organisation (WHO). Currently,
COVID-19 cases have been recorded globally (Rauf,
Et. Al., 2020). According to JHU CSSE Covid-19
data on 23 April 2021, reports indicated that
140,849,925 individuals were infected with the
disease of whom 3,013,217 died (Dong, Du, &
Gardner, 2020) .
Pandemics are "frequently marked by uncertainty,
confusion and a sense of urgency" (WHO, 2005).
Prior to, or in the early stages of a pandemic, there is
widespread uncertainty about the odds and
seriousness of becoming infected, along with
uncertainty, and possible misinformation, about the
best methods of prevention and management.
Uncertainty may persist well into the pandemic,
especially concerning the question of whether a
pandemic is truly over. Pandemics can come in
waves. Waves of infection are caused, in part, by
fluctuations in patterns of human aggregation, such as
seasonal movements of people away from, and then
458
Adelina, I., Handayani, V. and Megarini, M.
Systematic Literature Review on Mindset and the Benefits in Living New Normal Life.
DOI: 10.5220/0010754300003112
In Proceedings of the 1st International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences (ICE-HUMS 2021), pages 458-469
ISBN: 978-989-758-604-0
Copyright
c
2022 by SCITEPRESS – Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
into contact with, one another (Taylor, 2019)
Pandemics are related with a score of other
psychosocial stressors, counting wellbeing dangers to
oneself and others, extreme disturbances of schedule,
partition from family and friends, deficiencies of
nourishment and medication, loss, social isolation
because of quarantine or social distancing programs
and school closure, and individual budgetary
challenge. The personal financial impact of a
pandemic can be as severe and stressful as the
infection itself, especially for people who are already
experiencing financial difficulties (Taylor, 2019).
People differ in how they react to psychosocial
stressors such as the threat of, or an actual occurrence
of, a pandemic. Reactions can be diverse, ranging
from fear to indifference to fatalism. Some people
underestimate the risks, so they are less engaged in
recommended health behaviors such as vaccination,
hygiene practices, and social distancing. At the other
hand, many people react with intense anxiety or fear.
Actually, a moderate level of fear or anxiety can
motivate people to cope with health threats, but
severe distress can be debilitating (Taylor, 2019).
People develop beliefs that organize their world
and give meaning to their experiences. These beliefs
are called "meaning systems", and different people
will create different meaning systems. We have belief
systems that give structure to our world and meaning
to our experiences. People's beliefs about themselves
can create different psychological world, leading
them to think, feel, and act differently in identical
situations. Meaning systems are important in shaping
our thinking. The meaning systems that people
adopted were as important or even more important in
shaping their thinking (Dweck, 2000). A mindset is
defined as a mental frame or lens that selectively
organizes and encodes information, thereby orienting
an individual toward a unique way of understanding
an experience and guiding one toward corresponding
actions and responses (Crum, Salovey, & Achor,
2013).
Many studies examine how a person's mindset
affects the way he interprets and solves problems.
When facing challenging problems, people who
believe that effort drives intelligence tend to do better
than people who believe that intelligence is a fixed
quality that they cannot change. Individual with a
fixed mindset avoids challenges, gives up
effortlessly, sees effort as vain or more regrettable,
overlook valuable negative input, and feels
debilitated by the others succeeds. In the meantime,
people with a growth mindset embraces challenge,
persists despite setbacks, sees effort important to gain
mastery, learns from mistakes and criticisms, and
finds lessons and motivation in others success. People
with a growth mindset believe that they can develop
their abilities through hard work, persistence, and
dedication (Dweck, 2006; Elliot & Dweck, 2005;
Weiner, 2005). Research also suggests that good
problem solvers are qualitatively different from poor
problem solvers (National Research Council, 2004;
Schoenfeld, 2007). Good problem solvers are flexible
and resourceful. They have many ways to think about
problems, have alternative approaches if they get
stuck, ways of making progress when they hit
roadblocks, of being efficient with (and making use
of) what they know.
Other studies examine how the role of mindset in
dealing with stressful situations. Stress mindset is
related with psychological stress responses, through
coping strategies (Horiuchi, Tsuda, Aoki, Yoneda, &
Sawaguchi, 2018). Crum et al. (2013) found that
individuals with a stronger stress-is-enhancing
mindset utilized approach and active coping more
frequently and avoidant or withdrawal coping less
frequently. They showed that coping and stress
mindset were independently related with
psychological stress responses. Researches related to
the pandemic situation also turned out to provide
many findings on how mindset changes can help
overcome the pandemic situation. Therefore, the
purpose of this study is to integrate all of these
findings and explain them systematically and
thoroughly.
2 METHODS AND MATERIALS
This research based on descriptive methods in form
of systematic literature review. Articles related to this
literature review were searched through a computer-
based article data search program, the Google
Scholar, Scopus, and Proquest program. The
keywords are mindset, stress, and pandemic. All the
article findings were considered according to the
criteria as a requirement. The inclusion criteria for an
article to meet the requirements for analysis is that the
research contains a pandemic condition which
explains the mindset and stress variables. The
primary study was conducted using a survey that
examined the mindset. Based on the inclusion criteria
that have been set, it was found 50 research articles
started from 1994 and the following data were
processed into 22 studies. The research articles found
were taken from the Journal of Public Health
Management and Practice, Behavioral and Cognitive
Systematic Literature Review on Mindset and the Benefits in Living New Normal Life
459
Psychotherapy, Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, Journal of Psychiatry, Psychological
Review, International Journal of Health Management
and Information, American Journal of Public Health,
Stress and Health Journal, Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, Journal of Health Communication,
Journal of Behavioral Medicine, Psychiatry
Research, Clinical Psychology Review, and Journal
of Health Psychology. The results of the research
findings are then arranged into a table, analysed
through logical thinking, and then a conclusion is
drawn.
3 RESULT AND DISCUSSION
Research results can be found in table 1.
Table 1: Systematic literature review results.
No Researcher Keywords Results
1
L
oeb & Dweck
(
1994)
Stress,
mindset
People with growth mindset
tend to take a more direct
and active problem-solving
approach.
2
D
weck (2000) Stress,
mindset
Growth mindset may help us
to construct the lives we
want and to maintain the
flexibility to reconstruct
them when things go wrong.
3
T
aylor & Asmu
n
dson(2004)
Stress,
mindset,
health
Grossly inaccurate beliefs
can contribute to excessive
health anxiety.
4
T
aylor & Asmu
n
dson(2004);
W
heaton,
bramowitz,
B
erman,
F
abricant, &
O
latunji (2012)
Stess,
mindset
People with excessive health
anxiety tend to misinterpret
harmless bodily sensations.
5 WHO (2008,
2012); World
Health
organization
Writing Group
(2006)
Stress,
mindset,
pandemic
Psychological factors are
also relevant for under-
standing and addressing the
socially disruptive behavioral
patterns that can arise as a
result of widespread, serious
infection. Contemporary
methods for managing
pandemics are largely
behavioral or educational
interventions.
6
L
evi, Segal, St.
L
aurent, &
L
ieberman(2010)
Mindset,
pandemic
Attitudes about vaccination
are influenced by one's
b
eliefs about the vaccination.
7 Crum et al.,
(2013)
Stress,
mindset,
health
Stress mindset alters health-
related outcomes.
8 Dweck, (2017) Stress,
mindset
-
Several personality traits
have been linked to the
vulnerability to experience
negative emotions in
response to stressors.
-
Those with the growth
mindset, believe they can
develop their selves, open
to accurate information
about their current abilities,
oriented toward learning.
-
Cognitive therapy helps
people make more realistic
and optimistic judgments
into the framework of
growth.
9 Leventhal,
Phillips, &
Burns, (2016)
Stress,
mindset,
health
People can hold erroneous
beliefs about what is an
effective treatment
10 Taylor, (2017) Mindset,
pandemic
People with persistent
pandemic related PTSD would
likely benefit from empirically
supported treatments such as
trauma-focused CBT.
11 Gautreau et al
(2015); Hagger,
Koch,
Chatzisarantis,
& Orbell,
(2017)
Mindset,
stress
Cognitive-behavioral models
propose that excessive
anxiety about one's health is
triggered by the
misinterpretation of health-
related stimuli.
12 Cooper,Gregory,
Walker, Lambe
& Salkovskis,
(2017); Steven
Taylor & Asmu-
ndson, (2004)
Mindset,
stress
Cognitive-behavioral models
suggest that excessive health
anxiety can be addressed by
targeting dysfunctional
beliefs and maladaptive
b
ehaviors.
13 Taylor &
Asmundson(20
04); Tyrer &
Tyrer (2018)
Mindset,
stress
CBT, as conducted by a
therapist, is currently the first-
line treatment for excessive
health anxiety.
14 Tang, Bie,
Park, & Zhi,
(2018)
Mindset,
stress,
pandemic
Social media can fuel or quell
fears, and they can influence
the spreading of disease by
influencing people's behavior.
15 Keech,
Hagger, &
Hamilton
(2021)
Stress,
mindset
A stress-is enhancing mindset
can be induced through
intervention and have been
shown to be effective in
mitigating negative outcomes
to highly stressful events.
16 Taylor (2019) Mindset,
stress,
pandemic
- Cognitive and behavioral
factors play a role in
shaping the severity of
health anxiety:
Misinterpreta-tions of
health-related stimuli,
maladaptive or distorted
beliefs, memory and
attention processes, and
maladaptive behaviors
- Beliefs and fears about
diseases, just like diseases
themselves, spread through
social networks. Beliefs
and rumors also influence
the spread of infection.
Mindset is grounded on implicit theories, which
are knowledge structures about the malleability of an
ICE-HUMS 2021 - International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences
460
attribute such as intelligence and personality that
organize the way people ascribe meaning to events.
Research on implicit theories distinguishes between
two main beliefs or mindsets: an incremental or
growth mindset and an entity or fixed mindset
(Dweck, 2000; Dweck & Leggett, 1988) Those with
growth mindsets believe that human attributes are
malleable and therefore can be cultivated through
hard work, good strategies, and support from others.
They have a dynamic self and a dynamic world,
capable of growth. These beliefs help us move
forward with determination, encourage us to look for
ways to remedy our deficiencies and to solve our
problems. In contrast, other beliefs portray a more
static self and world with inherent, fixed qualities.
Those with fixed mindsets believe that human
attributes are fixed and therefore cannot be
developed, regardless of the effort expended or
strategy employed. These beliefs may have some
advantages, because they portray a simpler world that
is potentially easy to know, and there may be a great
deal of security in that. Entity beliefs can lead us to
make more rigid judgements, sometimes blinding
ourselves from our capabilities, and limiting the path
we pursue. Research finds that people can hold
different mindsets in different domains and the effects
are typically stronger for domain-specific
assessments (Scott & Ghinea, 2014). These beliefs
are part of people’s motivational systems. People’s
mindset has impact on their judgment, evaluations,
health, and behavior. Using one mindset or another
can significantly influence psychological, behavioral,
and physiological results in life and health domains
(Crum et al., 2013).
People tend to feel positive or negative emotions
because of the meaning they give to something that
has happened. Seligman and his colleagues set about
assessing individual differences in the kinds of causal
explanations that people tend to make for negative
events in their lives. They called this “explanatory
styles”. Some people tend to focus on more
pessimistic explanations for negative events, blaming
more global and stable factors, while other tend to
focus on more optimistic explanations, blaming more
specific and temporary ones (Dweck, 2000).
Cognitive-oriented theories of mental health and
psychotherapy start with the assumption that people’s
erroneous beliefs can get them into trouble. There are
series of beliefs that characterize individuals who are
vulnerable to emotional distress. Pessimistic
explanatory styles are also cognitive models of
vulnerability. While not denying biological
contributions to emotional disorders, research
therapies in this field show that many people with
depression or anxiety disorders are victims of their
maladaptive beliefs and alteration in these beliefs will
help them greatly (Dweck, 2000).
An entity theory framework can lead people to
overgeneralize from one experience, to categorize
themselves in unflattering ways, to set self-worth
contingencies, to exaggerate their failures relative to
their successes, to lose faith in their ability to perform
even simple actions, to underestimate the efficacy of
effort – all things that have been implicated in
depression. Pessimistic explanatory styles went on
the power of a helpless, and the power of an
optimistic explanatory styles to predict mental and
physical health (Dweck, 2000). Vulnerable people
don’t just think and react in different ways from less
vulnerable people. They also value different goals.
Compared with the less vulnerable people, they are
more concerned with validating themselves and less
concerned with growth and self-development
(Dweck, 2000).
Individual appraisal of the causes of stress will
determine their response. Research conducted by
Lazarus and Folkman, highlighted the importance of
cognitive appraisal in determining responses to stress.
The study proposes that individuals initially assess
the extent to which the situation is considered
demanding (primary appraisal) and then assess
whether they have sufficient resources or not to cope
with the situation (secondary appraisal). Recently,
researchers describe the stages of how individuals
assess a situation and highlight that the response to
stress is determined by the balance of perceived
resources (knowledge and skills), perceived demands
(danger and uncertainty), and the identification of the
physiological support for the challenges and threats
of these individual assessments (Crum, Akinola,
Martin, & Fath, 2017).
Stress mindset refers to the properties and desires
attributed to stress; coping refers to the process of
appraising threat and organizing cognitive and
behavioral resources to encounter stress when it does
occur. In other words, whereas stress mindset may
inform the coping strategy that one adapts, as the
mental and motivational situation in which coping
activity are chosen and occupied, it isn’t by itself a
coping strategy (Crum et al., 2013).
Mindsets plays an important role in stress
appraisals which will then determine individual’s
reactions to stressors are adaptive and point to
effective coping, or maladaptive and end in
ineffective coping and compromised health and
wellbeing. The main point of these concept is that
people who appraise stress as challenging and have
beliefs that stress can be enhancing and encouraging
Systematic Literature Review on Mindset and the Benefits in Living New Normal Life
461
interest of valued goals, cope more effectively and
show better outcomes. As opposed, people who
appraise stress as threatening, and have beliefs that
stress can be debilitating suboptimal in goal pursuit
(Hagger et al., 2017).
It is proposed that, when people feature a stress-
is-debilitating mindset, their arousal levels are likely
to be hypo- or hyperactivated. Arousal levels may be
hypoactive under stress as an impact of avoidance or
denial of the stress or the use of counteractive coping
mechanisms such as medications or substance use.
Alternatively, arousal levels may be hyperactivated
directly as a result of the additional stress that comes
from having a stress-is-debilitating mindset or
indirectly through counter-effective reactions of
emotional suppression, experiential avoidance, or
ruminative thought. Contrarily, people with stress is
enhancing mindset, more likely to attain an optimal
level of arousal when under stress, they have enough
arousal needed to fulfil goals and demands but not
exaggerated to debilitate physiological health at last.
Researches also show that changes in mindsets can
affect health through indirect changes in behavior and
physiology (Crum et al., 2013).
A stress-is-enhancing mindset is parallel with an
incremental perspective, such that individuals have a
flexible perspective on stress and have beliefs that
stress is an opportunity for growth with the potential
to facilitate performance and functioning (Crum et al.,
2013). In contrast, a stress-is debilitating mindset is
more in line with an entity perspective such that
people have a view that stress is harmful. A
developing research has shown that people with
stress-is-enhancing mindset experienced reduced
physiological stress responses, greater positive affect
and cognitive flexibility, better self-rated health,
higher life satisfaction, and better academic and work
performance (Crum et al., 2013). Furthermore,
research in various situation has shown that a stress-
is enhancing mindset can be induced through
intervention and shown effective result in relieving
highly stressful events (Crum et al., 2017, 2013;
Keech et al., 2021).
Researchers stated that stress might be beneficial,
at least up to a certain point. But once stress hits a
critical point or allostatic load, it becomes debilitating
(distress), pictured as an inverted-U-shaped curve
represent the relationship between arousal and
performance. The assumption that an objective level
of stress predicts physical and psychological results
largely has been obscured by the idea that responses
to stress are driven by how people manage or
anticipate the negative impacts of stress; in effect,
how—and how well—they adapt (Crum et al., 2013).
These beliefs can be influenced or changed by an
explicit message, or indirectly by other people’s
feedback (Dweck, 2006).
Coping preferences may grow out of meaning
systems. Some beliefs and goals may help us to
construct the lives we want and to maintain the
flexibility to reconstruct them when things go wrong.
Although most theories view coping as a process and
resist thinking in term of traits and rigid coping styles,
there has been identified more adaptive coping
strategies that tend to be more mastery-oriented,
active, and effective. They must adopt new goals, and
they must learn new strategies for attaining their
goals. Their successful adjustment depends on how
well this is done (Dweck, 2000).
The hypothetical supporting the suggestion that
stress mindset changes health and performance is that
different stress mindsets will be associated with
distinctive processes of motivational and
physiological. Specifically, it is said that stress
mindset has a significant impact on the manner in
which stress is behaviorally approached as well as the
manner in which stress is psychologically
experienced which these short-term impacts on
physiology and motivation have long-term impacts
on health and performance outcomes (Crum et al.,
2013). People with growth mindset tend to take more
direct problem-solving approach, while those with
fixed mindset tend to lost in negative feelings or turn
away from the problem and try to make themselves
feel better. Studies by Loeb & Dweck (1994) show a
similar thing. When confronted with scenarios
portraying them as victims, again, those with growth
mindset reported that they would take a more active
problem-solving stance, while those with fixed
mindset showed more passive acceptance but
admitted they would harbor long-term hatred and
wishes of revenge.
More particularly, in case people has a stress-is-
debilitating mindset, their primary motivation is to
avoid or manage the stress, preventing it from
becoming debilitating outcomes. On the other hand,
when one has a stress-is-enhancing mindset, their
primary motivation is to accept and utilize stress
toward achieving enhanced outcomes. As such, in the
event one has a stress is-debilitating mindset, one will
be more likely to engage in actions and coping
behaviors that act to avoid or manage the stress itself
(in an effort to prevent debilitating outcomes from
happening). On the other hand, if people have a
stress-is-enhancing mindset, they will more likely
engage in actions that help meet the demand, value,
or goal underlying the stressful situation (Crum et al.,
2013).
ICE-HUMS 2021 - International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences
462
Psychological factors play an important role in the
way in which people cope with the threat of pandemic
infection and its sequelae. Although many people
cope well under threat, many other people experience
high levels of distress or a worsening of pre-existing
psychological problems, such as anxiety disorders
and other clinical conditions. Psychological factors
are further important for understanding and managing
broader societal problems associated with pandemics,
such as factors involved in the spreading of excessive
fear. People may fear for their health, safety, family,
finances, or jobs. Psychological factors are also
important for understanding and managing the
potentially disruptive or maladaptive defensive
reactions, such as increases in stigmatization and
xenophobia that occur when people are threatened
with infection (Taylor, 2019). If examined deeply,
people behaviors during pandemic can be explained
on the mindset perspective.
Several personality traits have been linked to the
vulnerability to experience negative emotions in
response to stressors. These traits are interrelated and
transdiagnostic in that they are associated with a
range of emotional problems (Kring & Sloan, 2010;
Norton & Paulus, 2017). Every type describes
people’s behavior, but more importantly, is the
psychological reasons for people’s behavior – about
the beliefs and goals people bring to a situation that
caused them to act in certain ways (Dweck, 2000).
Negative emotionality, also known as neuroticism, is
the general tendency to become easily distressed by
aversive stimuli. People who scored high on this trait
tend to often experience aversive emotions such as
anxiety, irritability, and depression in response to
stressors (Costa & McCrae, 1987). Therefore, it is not
surprising that the severity of a person's negative
emotionality predicts their likelihood of becoming
distressed by the threat of infection. They tend to
overestimate of threat. People who score high on
overestimation threat tend to overestimate the cost
("badness") and probability (likelihood) of aversive
events, and see themselves as being especially
vulnerable to threats (Frost & Steketee, 2020). The
impact is they are likely to become highly worried
and anxious because their estimates of being harmed
tend to be inflated compared to the estimates of
people scoring lower on these traits. The intolerance
of uncertainty is another facet or sub-trait of trait
anxiety that can contribute to the tendency to
experience anxiety and fear (McEvoy & Mahoney,
2013). They have a strong desire for predictability.
When faced with important uncertainties, these
people might feel paralyzed with indecision (Birrell,
Meares, Wilkinson, & Freeston, 2011). During the
pandemics, the intolerance of uncertainty is likely to
be a particularly important contributor to pandemic-
related anxiety and distress. During times of
pandemics, people need to be able to tolerate or
accept a certain degree of uncertainty. People who are
unable or unwilling to accept uncertainty are likely to
experience considerable distress. People with a high
degree of intolerance of uncertainty tend to become
highly anxious about the threat of infectious disease,
especially if they perceive themselves as having
limited control over the threat (Taha, Matheson,
Cronin, & Anisman, 2014)
Unlike the above-mentioned traits, which are
associated with negative beliefs or expectations, the
unrealistic optimism bias is associated with persistent
and unrealistically positive beliefs about one's future
(Taylor & Brown, 1988). Optimism-defined as the
hope that something good is going to happen (Carver,
Scheier, & Segerstrom, 2010) can be a state variable
or an enduring personality trait. Optimism trait, which
is our focus here, is negatively correlated with
negative emotionality, although the correlation is far
from perfect (Kam & Meyer, 2012). Regardless of
these theoretical debates, people scoring low on traits
such as negative emotionality generally tend toward
optimism. Many people, although the precise
prevalence is unknown, have an unrealistic optimism
bias (Makridakis & Moleskis, 2015). This is the
strong tendency to believe that positive events are
more likely to happen to themselves than to others,
and that negative events are more likely to happen to
other people than themselves. Such people tend to
undervalue dangers such as diseases and other
hardships, whose existence they accept but cannot
believe will happen to themselves (Makridakis &
Moleskis, 2015). People with strong unrealistic
optimism bias tend to see themselves as impervious
to infection (Ji, Zhang, Usborne, & Guan, 2004; Kim
& Niederdeppe, 2013). In the event of a pandemic,
the unrealistic optimism bias can have deleterious
effects. It may lead people to underestimate their
susceptibility to risk, thereby reducing attention to
risk information and leading them to neglect to do
preventive health behaviors such as seeking
vaccination (Kim & Niederdeppe, 2013). The
unrealistic optimism bias can be resistant to change in
the face of disconfirming information (Sharot, Korn,
& Dolan, 2011). Related to the unrealistic optimism
bias is the sense of invulnerability. That is, the sense
that one is unlikely to be affected by threats such as
serious infectious disease. People with an inflated
sense of invulnerability are (1) less likely to
experience anxiety in response to stressful life events;
(2) more likely to take up smoking or drug use; (3)
Systematic Literature Review on Mindset and the Benefits in Living New Normal Life
463
more likely to drink and drive; and (4) less likely to
intend to seek vaccination, even for pandemics such
as Swine flu (Taylor, 2019). During the next
pandemic, people with strong unrealistic optimism
bias or a strong sense of invulnerability will probably
be less worried than other people and possibly more
likely to spread infection by failing to seek
vaccination and by neglecting to do basic hygiene
behaviors such as hand washing.
Maybe the people with the growth mindset more
likely to have expanded sees of their capacity and try
for things they’re not capable of? In truth, Researches
show that people are terrible at estimating their
abilities. But people with the fixed mindset who
accounted for almost all the inaccuracy. The people
with growth mindset were amazingly accurate
(Dweck, 2017). Those with growth mindset, believe
they can develop their selves, open to accurate
information about their current abilities, even if it’s
unflattering. What’s more, in case they’re situated
toward learning, they require exact information about
their current abilities to learn effectively. However, if
everything is either good news or bad news about
their precious qualities —as it is with fixed mindset
people—distortion almost inevitably enters the
picture. Some outcomes are magnified, others are
ignored, and before they realize it, they become
unrealistic.
Health anxiety refers to the tendency to become
alarmed by illness related stimuli, including but not
limited to, illness related to infectious diseases.
Health anxiety ranges on a continuum from mild to
severe, and can be a state or a trait. The latter is a
relatively enduring tendency. Our focus is on trait
health anxiety. Some people have very low levels of
health anxiety. Their lack of concern about health
risks can be maladaptive (e.g., neglecting to take
necessary health precautions). Excessively low health
concerns can be associated with an unrealistic
optimism bias, as discussed before. People who are
unconcerned about infection tend to neglect to do
recommended hygienic behaviors, such as washing
their hands after using the washroom and tend to be
nonadherent to social distancing (Taylor, 2019).
Excessively high health anxiety is characterized
by undue anxiety or worry about one's health. That is,
a disproportionate concern, given one's objective
level of health. People with excessively high levels of
health anxiety, compared to less anxious people, tend
to become unduly alarmed by all kinds of perceived
health threats, and overestimate the likelihood and
seriousness of becoming ill (Hedman et al., 2016).
Excessive health anxiety is associated with high
levels of functional impairment and high levels of
health care service utilization, even after controlling
for physical comorbidities (Bobevski, Clarke, &
Meadows, 2016; Eilenberg, Frostholm, Schroder,
Jensen, & Fink, 2015). Excessive health anxiety-as
seen in psychiatric disorders such as hypochondriasis,
illness anxiety disorder, and somatic symptom
disorder is common, with an estimated lifetime
prevalence of 6% in the community (Sunderland,
Newby, & Andrews, 2013). People prone to
excessive health anxiety are likely to become
particularly anxious during a threatened or real
epidemic or pandemic. Such people may misinterpret
somatic stress reactions (e.g., sweating, hot flushes,
increased muscle tension) as signs of infection.
Furthermore, they can experience the nocebo effect,
which occurs when negative expectations about
treatment (e.g., a vaccination injection) cause the
patient to experience negative side effects (Taylor,
2019). Traits such as negative emotionality
(neuroticism) may predispose people to experience
the nocebo effect (Data-Franco & Berk, 2013).
Interpretations of health-related stimuli are
influenced by memory processes such as
recollections of past experiences and by longstanding
beliefs (Salkovskis & Warwick, 2001; Taylor &
Asmundson, 2004). Learning experiences (e.g.,
experiences of being hospitalized as a child) can lead
some people to mistakenly believe that their health is
fragile (Taylor & Asmundson, 2004). People with
excessive health anxiety tend to believe that all bodily
sensations or bodily changes are potential signs of
disease (Taylor & Asmundson, 2004) In the case of
influenza, grossly inaccurate beliefs can contribute to
excessive health anxiety. Such beliefs are
unfortunately commonplace. Attentional processes
are important cognitive factors in shaping the
intensity of health anxiety (Norris & Marcus, 2014).
People with excessive health anxiety tend to be
hypervigilant to bodily changes and sensations; that
is, they pay a lot of attention to their bodies and
therefore are likely to notice benign bodily
perturbations. This selective attention increases the
odds of noticing bodily changes or sensations. The
fear of infection led people to persistently focus on
their bodies, leading many people to misinterpret
benign bodily changes or sensations. Selective
attention to bodily states is influenced not only by
internal factors (i.e., sensations, beliefs,
expectations), but also by external stimuli (Taylor,
2019)
People's interpretations influence whether or not
they seek treatment, and whether they seek
appropriate treatment. People can hold erroneous
beliefs about what is an effective treatment Some
ICE-HUMS 2021 - International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences
464
people believe that they only need symptomatic relief
(e.g., cough suppressant medications), which may be
insufficient if the underlying disease needs to be
treated (Leventhal et al., 2016). People's appraisals of
risk are often inaccurate. Indeed, there is only a weak
correlation between people's anxiety about a
particular risk and objective probability of death or
harm (Frost, Frank, & Maibach, 1997; Young,
Norman, & Humphreys, 2008). People with high
levels of health anxiety sometimes regard clinics as a
source of sickness rather than a resource for help.
People with excessive anxiety about infection tend to
engage in maladaptive safety behaviors (i.e.,
behaviors intended to keep themselves safe) such as
excessive hand washing and repeatedly seeking
reassurance from medical professionals. Excessive
handwashing can impair functioning in other areas of
life (e.g., occupational functioning), especially when
people devote hours per day to unnecessary
handwashing. Excessive reassurance-seeking (e.g.,
repeatedly and unnecessarily seeking assurances that
one is not sick) can add an unnecessary burden on the
healthcare system. Excessive reassurance-seeking
can also perpetuate health anxiety because (1) it
increases the risk that the person will obtain
conflicting medical information, (2) increases the risk
of iatrogenic interventions, and (3) reinforces the
person's view that their health is at risk (Taylor,
2019). The latter can occur, for example, when
unnecessary medical tests (e.g., laboratory tests) are
given in an attempt to reassure the anxious patient
The testing can be misinterpreted by the patient.
Reassurance-seeking can consist of persistent
searching the Internet for medical information
("cyberchondria"; (Mathes, Norr, Allan, Albanese, &
Schmidt, 2018), which increases the odds that the
person will be exposed to alarming, false information
(Taylor & Asmundson, 2004). People with excessive
health anxiety also tend to engage in "doctor
shopping"; that is, seeking consultations with
multiple physicians so as to reassure themselves that
they are not suffering from a serious disease. Doctor
shopping places an undue burden on the medical
system and increases the chances that the patient will
receive seemingly conflicting or confusing medical
advice (Taylor & Asmundson, 2004).
Beliefs and fears about diseases, just like diseases
themselves, spread through social networks. Beliefs
also influence the spread of infection. If there is
widespread belief in the importance of handwashing.
for example, then this will curtail the spread of
disease. In general, beliefs and fears are spread in
three main ways: (1) Information transmission, such
as by media reports (e.g., text, images) or verbal
information received from other people (e.g.,
rumors); (2) direct personal experiences, including
conditioning events (e.g., exposure to trauma); and
(3) observational learning (e.g., witnessing other
people acting frightened in response to some
stimulus). Information transmission and
observational learning are particularly relevant to the
spread of beliefs and fears through social networks (
Taylor, 2019).
A rumor, as the term is defined in the social
sciences, refers to a "story or piece of information of
unknown reliability that is passed from person to
person. Rumors are "improvised news", spreading
rapidly when the demand for information exceeds the
supply, as is the case during times of uncertainty
about important issues. Rumors may be spread if they
help people make sense of an ambiguous situation,
such as the possible threat of infection, and if rumors
offer guidance about how to cope with the perceived
risks (DiFonzo & Bordia, 2007). Rumors can arise
from anonymous sources, causing uncertainty about
the veracity of the information. Rumors can be spread
maliciously and to promote prejudice.
Social media have become a major source of
health information for people worldwide and have
become a global platform for outbreak and health risk
communication (Taylor, 2019). Social media are a
two-edged sword. They can rapidly disseminate
information and misinformation. They can fuel or
quell fears, and they can influence the spreading of
disease by influencing people's behavior. This
potentially raises problems with the spreading of
excessive fear. The same can be said for modern
communication technologies in general, including the
Internet. A large volume of misleading information is
posted on social media. Research indicates, for
example, that about 20-30% of You Tube videos
about emerging infectious diseases contain inaccurate
or misleading information (Tang et al., 2018).
Emotional contagion, including the spread of fear,
is a basic building block of human interaction,
allowing people to understand and share the feelings
of others by "feeling themselves into" another
person's emotions (Hatfield, Carpenter, & Rapson,
2014). Research shows that observational learning is
an important way in which emotions, including fears,
are spread (Bandura, 1986). Observational learning
involves the acquisition of information, skills, or
behavior by watching the performance of others.
Fears may be acquired via observational learning,
such as by seeing or hearing people express fear about
some issue, such as a possible pandemic.
Observational learning can include seeing fearful faces
or bodily postures and hearing frightened voices.
Systematic Literature Review on Mindset and the Benefits in Living New Normal Life
465
Contemporary methods for managing pandemics
are largely behavioral or educational interventions-
that is, vaccination adherence programs, hygienic
practices, and social distancing-in which
psychological factors play a vital role. Excessive
emotional distress associated with threatened or
actual infection is a further issue of clinical and public
health significance. Psychological factors are also
relevant for understanding and addressing the socially
disruptive behavioral patterns that can arise as a result
of widespread, serious infection. Four main methods
are used to manage the spread of infection: (1) Risk
communication (public education), (2) vaccines and
antiviral therapies, (3) hygiene practices, and (4)
social distancing (WHO, 2008, 2012; World Health
organization Writing Group, 2006). Psychological
factors play an essential role in the success of each of
these methods.
Pharmacological Treatments Vaccines and
antiviral medications are the primary
pharmacological methods for managing pandemic
influenza. The development of vaccines for infectious
diseases is a time consuming, costly business, with a
more than 90% failure rate (Gouglas et al., 2018).
Psychological factors, specifically mindset, are
important for understanding seemingly self-defeating
behaviors such as vaccination nonadherence (Taylor,
2019). In terms of influenza, people are unlikely to
seek vaccination if they (1) believe (accurately or not)
that they are unlikely to be exposed to an influenza
virus, (2) see themselves as being impervious to
infection, (3) do not perceive the infection to be a
serious problem, (4) perceive that there are significant
inconveniences or barriers to adherence, and (5) have
misgivings about the safety and efficacy of
vaccination (Taylor, 2019). People with very strong
beliefs about negative side effects may refuse to be
vaccinated even though they might also acknowledge
that the infection is potentially dangerous.
Vaccination hesitancy is a widespread, important
problem, even among medical practitioners and even
during times of pandemics. Various types of negative
attitudes and other psychological factors appear to
play a role, such as psychological reactance, PVD,
and injection phobia. Treating the attitudinal and
motivational roots of the problem may be vital during
the pandemic. Public education campaigns show
promise as do interventions targeting particular
problems such as injection phobia. Mandatory
vaccination as a requirement for employment may be
viable for medical practitioners and workers in other
sectors. It is unclear whether mandatory vaccination
would be viable on a community-wide level (Taylor,
2019).
Hygiene Practices Commonly recommended
hygiene practices include handwashing with soap or
hand sanitizer, covering sneezes/coughs (e.g.,
sneezing into the crook of one's arm), hand awareness
(i.e., refraining from touching one's eyes, nose or
mouth), cleaning household surfaces, and wearing
facemasks (WHO, 2008). Social Distancing refers to
interventions, either recommended or mandated by
health authorities, to reduce the probability that
infected people will spread disease to others
(Finkelstein, Prakash, Nigmatulina, Klaiman, &
Larson, 2010). Social distancing can include some or
all of the following, depending on the severity of an
outbreak: Quarantine of infected persons, school
closure, workplace closure, cancelling mass
gatherings such as sporting events and concerts,
closing recreational facilities (e.g., community
centers), closing non-essential businesses (e.g., clubs
and bars), cancelling non-essential domestic travel,
self-imposed isolation of uninfected people (e.g.,
remaining home, when possible), and border and
travel restrictions (World Health organization
Writing Group, 2006). Mindset predict a person's
proclivity to engage in the hygiene behaviors and
social distancing necessary for pandemic control.
Changing people’s mindset through the
communication message delivered communication
guidelines are as follows: 1. Announce the outbreak
early, even with incomplete information, so as to
minimize the spread of rumors and misinformation.
2. Provide information about what the public can do
to make themselves safer. 3. Maintain transparency to
ensure public trust 4. Demonstrate that efforts are
being made to understand the public's views and
concerns about the outbreak. 5. Evaluate the impact
of communication programs to ensure that the
messages are being correctly understood and that the
advice is being followed (WHO, 2005, 2008).
In the event of disaster such as a pandemic, a lack
of mental health and social support systems and a lack
of well-trained mental health professionals can
increase the risk that people will develop emotional
and other forms of psychological disorders (Taylor,
2019). A proactive response is required, involving a
rapid assessment of outbreak-associated
psychological stressors, for both civilians and
medical practitioners. But even at the best of times,
busy medical practitioners, such as primary care
physicians, often fail to detect psychological
disorders. The situation is even more challenging
during a pandemic, where there is an increase in the
number of sick people and likely staff shortages due
to illness. Accordingly, there need to be efficient
procedures for identifying people who are at risk for,
ICE-HUMS 2021 - International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences
466
or actually suffering from, clinically significant
distress. Procedures are also needed for selecting
optimal interventions. The screen-and-treat method is
one such approach (Taylor, 2019).
Psychological interventions can be useful in the
early stages of a pandemic, when anticipatory anxiety
and worry are likely to be high, and in later stages,
especially where people are exposed to traumatic
events such as witnessing the death of friends and
loved ones. Psychological interventions can be useful
even after the pandemic has passed.
Mindsets outline the running account that’s taking
put in people’s heads. They guide the whole
interpretation process. In several studies, we probed
the way people with a fixed mindset dealt with
information they were receiving. We found that they
put a very strong evaluation on each and every piece
of information. Something good led to a very strong
positive label and something bad led to a very strong
negative label. The fixed mindset creates an internal
monologue that is focused on judging. Stronger
beliefs in the negative effects of stress, instead of the
positive effects of stress, were related with people’s
choice of emotional expression, which was in turn
associated with higher levels of irritation anger
(Horiuchi et al., 2018).
People with a growth mindset are too
continuously observing what’s going on, but their
inside monologue is not about judging themselves
and others in this way. Certainly, they’re delicate to
positive and negative information, but they’re
adjusted to its implications for learning and
constructive action. With growth mindset, people can
look more closely at the facts by asking: What is the
evidence for and against your conclusion? People
may also be encouraged to think of reasons of their
failure, and these may further temper their negative
judgment. In this way, people can get more realistic
and have more optimistic judgments to deal with their
situations in more adaptive ways and in turn generate
more positive and effective results.
4 CONCLUSIONS
In terms of theoretical insight, this study provides
results that mindset plays an important role in
understanding and addressing the socially disruptive
behavioral patterns that can arise as a result of
widespread, serious infection. Mindset are also
relevant for stress appraisals which will then
determine individual’s responses to stressors are
adaptive and lead to effective coping, or maladaptive
and lead to ineffective coping and compromised
health and functioning. Individuals with a growth
mindset were more likely to appraise a potential
stressor as challenging. These individuals were less
likely to be stressed, more likely to report positive
experiences, such as positive emotions, and use more
approach and active coping when they encountered
potentially stressful events. By contrast, individuals
with fixed mindset were more stressed, reported
negative experiences such as negative emotion, and
tend to use avoidant coping in stressful events.
REFERENCES
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and
action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs:
Prentice-Hall.
Birrell, J., Meares, K., Wilkinson, A., & Freeston, M.
(2011). Toward a definition of intolerance of
uncertainty: A review of factor analytical studies of the
intolerance of uncertainty scale. Clinical Psychology
Review, 31(7), 1198–1208. https://doi.org/doi:
l0.l016/j.cpr.2011.07.009
Bobevski, I., Clarke, D. M., & Meadows, G. (2016). Health
anxiety and its relationship to disability and service use:
Findings from a large epidemiological survey
Psychosomatic Medicine, 78(1), 13-25. Psychosomatic
Medicine, 78(1), 13–25. https://doi.org/doi: l0.l097/
PSY.0000000000000252
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2010).
Optimism. Clinical Psychology Review,30(7),879–889.
Cooper, K., Gregory, J. D., Walker, I., Lambe, S., &
Salkovskis, P. M. (2017). Cognitive behavior therapy
for health anxiety: A systematic review and meta-
analysis. Behavioral and Cognitive Psychotherapy,
45(2), 110–123. https://doi.org/doi:10.1017 /S13524
65817000510
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1987). Neuroticism, somatic
complaints, and disease: Is the bark worse than the bite?
Journal of Personality, 55(2), 299–316.
Crum, A. J., Akinola, M., Martin, A., & Fath, S. (2017).
The Role of Stress Mindset in Shaping Cognitive,
Emotional, and Physiological Responses to
Challenging and Threatening Stress. Anxiety, Stress, &
Coping, 30(4), 379–395. /https://doi.org/10.1080/106
15806.2016.1275585
Crum, A. J., Salovey, P., & Achor, S. (2013). Rethinking
stress: The role of mindsets in determining the stress
response. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 104(4), 716–733. https://psycnet.apa.
org/doi/10.1037/a0031201
Data-Franco, J., & Berk, M. (2013). The nocebo effect: A
clinician guide. Australian and New Zealand Journal of
Psychiatry, 47(7), 617–623. https://doi.org/doi:l0.1177
/0004867412464717
DiFonzo, N., & Bordia, P. (2007). Rumor psychology:
Social and organizational approaches. Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association.
Systematic Literature Review on Mindset and the Benefits in Living New Normal Life
467
Dong, E., Du, H., & Gardner, L. (2020). An interactive
web-based dashboard to track COVID-19 in real time.
Correspondence, 20(5), 533–534. https://doi.org/10.10
16/S1473-3099(20)30120-1
Dweck, C. (2000). Self theories: Their role in motivation,
personality, and development psychology. New York:
Taylor and Francis Group.
Dweck, C. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of
success. New York: Random House.
Dweck, C. (2017). Mindset: Changing the Way You Think
to Fulfil Your Potential(1
st
ed.). New York: Robinson
Ltd.
Dweck, C., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive
approach to motivation and personality. Psychological
Review, 95(2), 256–273. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-
295X.95.2.256
Eilenberg, T., Frostholm, L., Schroder, A., Jensen, J. S., &
Fink, P. (2015). Long-term consequences of severe
health anxiety on sick leave in treated and untreated
patients: Analysis alongside a randomised controlled
trial. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 32, 95–102.
https://doi.org/doi: 1 0.1 0 16 /j .janxdis.2015.04.001
Elliot, A., & Dweck, C. (2005). Handbook of competence
and motivation. New York: Guilford Press.
Finkelstein, S., Prakash, S., Nigmatulina, K., Klaiman, T.,
& Larson, R. (2010). Pandemic influenza: Non-
pharmaceutical interventions and behavioral changes
that may save lives. International Journal of Health
Management and Information, 1(1), 1–18.
Frost, K., Frank, E., & Maibach, E. (1997). Relative risk in
the news media: A quantification of misrepresentation.
American Journal of Public Health, 87(5), 842–845.
Frost, R. O., & Steketee, G. (2020). Cognitive approaches
to obsessions and compulsions: Theory, assessment,
and treatment. Oxford: Elsevier.
Gautreau, C. M., Sherry, S. B., Sherry, D. L., Birnie, K. .,
Mackinnon, S. P., & Stewart, S. H. (2015). Does
catastrophizing of bodily sensations maintain health-
related anxiety? A 14-day daily diary study with
longitudinal follow-up. Behavioral and Cognitive
Psychotherapy, 43(4), 502–512. https://doi.org/
doi:10.1017 /S1352465814000150
Gouglas, D., Le, T. T., Henderson, K., Kaloudis, A.,
Danielsen, T., Hammersland, N. ., & Rottingen, J. .
(2018). Estimating the cost of vaccine developent
against epidemic infectious diseases: A cost mini-
misation study. Lancet Global Health, 6(12), 1386-
1396. https://doi.org/doi:10.1016/S2214-109X(18)30
346-2
Hagger, M. S., Koch, S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Orbell,
S. (2017). The common sense model of self-regulation:
Meta-analysis and test of a process model.
Psychological Bulletin, 143(11), 1117–1154. https://
doi.org/doi: l0.l037/buI0000118
Hatfield, E., Carpenter, M., & Rapson, R. L. (2014).
Emotional contagion as a precursor to collective
emotions. In C. Von Scheve & M. Salmela (Eds.),
Collective emotions: Perspectives from psychology,
philosophy, and sociology (pp. 108–122). Oxford
University.
Hedman, E., Lekander, M., Karshikoff, B., Ljotsson, B.,
Axelsson, E., & Axelsson, J. (2016). Health anxiety in
a disease-avoidance framework: Investigation of anxiety,
disgust and disease perception in response to sickness
cues. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(7), 868–878.
https://doi.org/doi:10.1037/abn0000 195
Horiuchi, S., Tsuda, A., Aoki, S., Yoneda, K., &
Sawaguchi, Y. (2018). Coping as a mediator of the
relationship between stress mindset and psychological
stress response: a pilot study. Psychology Research and
Behavior Management, 1(11), 47–54. https://doi.org/d
oi: 10.2147/PRBM.S150400
Ji, L. ., Zhang, Z., Usborne, E., & Guan, Y. (2004).
Optimism across cultures: In response to the severe
acute respiratory syndrome outbreak. Asian Journal of
Social Psychology, 7, 25-34., 2, 25-34. https://doi.org
/doi:1 0.1111/j.146 7 -839X.2 004.00132.x
Kam, C., & Meyer, J. P. (2012). Do optimism and
pessimism have different relationships with personality
dimensions? A re-examination. Personality and
Individual Differences, 52(2), 123–127. https://doi
.org/doi:1 0.1016/j. paid.2011.09.011
Keech, J. J., Hagger, M. S., & Hamilton, K. (2021).
Changing stress mindsets with a novel imagery
intervention: A randomized controlled trial. Emotion,
21(1), 123–136. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1
037/emo 0000678
Kim, H. K., & Niederdeppe, J. (2013). Exploring optimistic
bias and the integrative model of behavioral prediction
in the context of a campus influenza outbreak. Journal
of Health Communication, 18(2), 206–222. https://
doi.org/doi:1 0.1080/10810730.2012.688247
Kring, A. M., & Sloan, M. D. (2010). Emotion regulation
and psychopathology: A transdiagnostic approach to
etiology and treatment. New York: Guilford Press.
Leventhal, H., Phillips, L. A., & Burns, E. (2016). The
common-sense model of self-regulation (CSM): A
dynamic framework for understanding illness self-
management. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 39(6),
935-946.https://doi.org/doi:10.1007/s10865-016-9782-2
Levi, J., Segal, L. M., St. Laurent, R., & Lieberman, D. A.
(2010). Fighting flu fatigue. Retrieved April 7, 2019,
from healthyamericans. org/ assets / fil es /TF AH 2 010
FI uBriefFI NAL. pdf.
Loeb, I. S., & Dweck, C. (1994). Beliefs about human
nature as predictors of reaction to victimization. The
Annual Convention of the American Psychological
Society. Washington, DC.
Makridakis, S., & Moleskis, A. (2015). The costs and
benefits of positive illusions. Frontiers in Psychology,
6. https://doi.org/doi:l0.3389/fpsyg.2015.00859
Mathes, B. M., Norr, A. M., Allan, N. P., Albanese, B. ., &
Schmidt, N. B. (2018). Cyberchondria: Overlap with
health anxiety and unique relations with impairment,
quality of life, and service utilization. Psychiatry
Research, 261, 204-211. doi:l 0.1016/j. psychres.2
018.01.002. Psychiatry Research, 261, 204-211.
https://doi.org/doi:l 0.1016/j. psychres.2 018.01.002
McEvoy, P. M., & Mahoney, A. E. J. (2013). Intolerance of
uncertainty and negative metacognitive beliefs as
ICE-HUMS 2021 - International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences
468
transdiagnostic mediators of repetitive negative
thinking in a clinical sample with anxiety disorders.
Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 27(2), 216-224.
https://doi.org/doi: 1 0.1 0 16 /j .janxdis. 2 0 13.01.006
National Research Council. (2004). How students learn
mathematics in the classroom. Washington, DC:
National Academies Press.
Norris, A. L., & Marcus, D. K. (2014). Cognition in health
anxiety and hypochondriasis: Recent advances. Current
Psychiatry Reviews, 10(1), 44–49. https://doi.org/
doi:10.2174/1573400509666131119004151
Norton, P. J., & Paulus, D. J. (2017). Transdiagnostic
models of anxiety disorder: Theoretical and empirical
underpinnings. Clinical Psychology Review, 56, 122–
137. https://doi.org/doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.03.004
Salkovskis, P. M., & Warwick, H. M. C. (2001). Meaning,
misinterpretations, and medicine: A cognitive-
behavioral approach to understanding health anxiety
and hypochondriasis. In V. Starcevic & D. R. Lipsitt
(Eds.), Hypochondria (pp. 202–222). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Schoenfeld, A. (2007). Assessing mathematical
proficiency. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Scott, M. ., & Ghinea, G. (2014). Measuring enrichment:
the assembly and validation of an instrument to assess
student self-beliefs in CS1. ICER ’14: Proceedings of
the Tenth Annual Conference on International
Computing Education Research., 123–130. https://doi.
org/DOI:10.1145/2632320.2632350
Sharot, T., Korn, C. W., & Dolan, R. J. (2011). How
unrealistic optimism is maintained in the face of reality.
Nature Neuroscience, 14(11), 1475–1479. https://doi.
org/doi: l0.l038/nn.2949
Sunderland, M., Newby, J. M., & Andrews, G. (2013).
Health anxiety in Australia: Prevalence, comorbidity,
disability and service use. British Journal of Psychiatry,
202(1), 56–61. https://doi.org/doi:1 0.1192/bjp.bp.lll.l0
3960
Taha, S., Matheson, K., Cronin, T., & Anisman, H. (2014).
Intolerance of uncertainty, appraisals, coping, and
anxiety: The case of the 2009 HINl pandemic. British
Journal of Health Psychology, 19(3), 592–605.
https://doi.org/doi:10.1111/bjhp.12058
Tang, L., Bie, B., Park, S. E., & Zhi, D. (2018). Social
media and outbreaks of emerging infectious diseases: A
systematic review of literature. American Journal of
Infection Control, 46(9), 962–972. https://doi.org/doi: 1
0.1016 /j.aj ic.2 018.02.010
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-
being: A social psychological perspective on mental
health. Psychological Bulletin, 103(2), 193–210.
https://doi.org/doi: l0.l037 /0033- 2909.103.2.193
Taylor, S. (2017). Clinician’s guide to PTSD (Second).
New York: Guildford.
Taylor, S. (2019). The Psychology of pandemics: Preparing
for the next global outbreak of infectious disease.
Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
Taylor, Steven, & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2004). Treating
health anxiety. New York: Guilford. New York:
Guilford Press.
Tyrer, P., & Tyrer, H. (2018). Health anxiety: Detection and
treatment. British Journal of Psychiatry Advances,
24(1), 66–72. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1192/
bja.2017.5
Weiner, B. (2005). Motivation from an attributional
perspective and the social psychology. In Handbook of
Competence and Motivation (pp. 73–84). New York:
Guilford Press.
Wheaton, M. G., Abramowitz, J. S., Berman, N. C.,
Fabricant, L. E., & Olatunji, B. O. (2012).
Psychological predictors of anxiety in response to the
HINl (swine flu) pandemic. Cognitive Therapy and
Research, 36(3), 210–218. https://doi.org/doi:l0.l007
/s10608-011-9353-3
WHO. (2005). WHO checklist for influenza pandemic
preparedness planning. Geneva.
WHO. (2008). WHO outbreak communication planning
guide. Geneva.
WHO. (2012). Vaccines against influenza WHO position
paper. Weekly Epidemiological Record, 87, 461–476.
World Health organization Writing Group. (2006).
Nonpharmaceutical interventions for pandemic
influenza, national and community measures.
Emerging infectious diseases.
Young, M. E., Norman, G. R., & Humphreys, K. R. (2008).
Medicine in the popular press: The influence of the
media on perceptions of disease. PLOS ONE, 3, E3552.,
3552(3). https://doi.org/Doi: l0.1371/journal.pone.000
3552
Systematic Literature Review on Mindset and the Benefits in Living New Normal Life
469