Situation Awareness Viewed from Sense of Direction and Choice
of Navigation Direction
M. Dinah Charlota Lerik
1
, T. Dicky Hastjarjo
2
, Rini Dharmastiti
3
1
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Public Health, University of Nusa Cendana, Kupang,, Indonesia
2
Faculty of Psychology, University of Gadjah Mada,Yogyakarta,, Indonesia
3
Ergonomics Laboratory, Department of Engineering and Industry, Faculty of Engineering,University of Gadjah Mada,
Yogyakarta, Indonesia
Keywords: Situation Awareness, Sense of Direction, Map and Verbal Direction.
Abstract: The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of sense of direction, map or verbal direction and
gender on situation awareness. One hundred and sixty five (165) students, consisting of 120 females and 45
males, participated in the study. Their ages between 18-19 years old. One-way anova was used to analyse
data. It was found that the choice of direction (map, verbal or both combination) had influence on situation
awareness level 1 and 3. However, sense of direction did not affect situation awareness. Participant gender
was found to influence the level 3 situation awareness but not of level 1 and 2. Women showed higher
situation awareness of level 3 than men.
1 INTRODUCTION
Every day people are adventurous in this world,
either on foot or using a vehicle, walking through
familiar routes or in unfamiliar areas. Some people
enter a new spatial location with anxiety about being
misdirected or lost, some people are even happy and
trying to find experiences to find a new location with
the help of directions such as maps and verbal
instructions, and some others instead just enjoy the
sensation of environmental spatial awareness and
body orientation in the environment.
Sense of Direction is operationalized as the
ability to position themselves and self-orientation in
the environment (Hegarty, Richardson, Montello,
Lovelace, & Subiah, 2002), and there is predictive
evidence from the use of self-report measurement of
sense direction in this psychological construct.
Previous research has shown that sense of direction
self-report is positively correlated with smoothness
in: (1) distance estimation (r = 0.00-0.480, n = 24-
286; Hegarty et al. 2002; Ishikawa & Montello,
2006); (2) direction estimation under various
conditions (r = 0.36-0.45, n = 22-25; Hegarty et al.,
2002; Ishikawa & Montello, 2006; Montello & Pick,
1993); (3) giving, following and remembering
directions (Hund & Padgit, 2010); (4) managing
orientation accuracy in complex environments (r =
0.51-0.82, n = 12-31; Sholl, Kenny, & DellaPorta,
2006); and perhaps most importantly, (5) accuracy
of finding locations (Hund & Padgit, 2010; Kato &
Tekeuchi, 2003).
This study examines the correlation between
sense of direction and choice of directions with
situation awareness. Situation awareness is defined
as the perception of environmental elements in the
volume of time and space, the arrangement of
meaning and the projected status of these elements
in the near future (Endsley, 1988). This definition is
similar to the definition of "spatial awareness"
explained by Klippel, Hirtle, & Davies (2010),
which states that situation awareness is part of
survey knowledge and is defined as the ability to
plan new routes, shortcuts, and detours in an
environment. This ability requires directional
sensitivity as defined by Kozlowski & Bryant (1977)
as awareness of location or orientation, especially
where a person knows where he is while he moves
around an environment.
Situation awareness is a theoretical concept and
measurement paradigm that arises from applied
research in individuals with certain expertise (eg air
traffic control officers) who are tasked with
managing constant vigilance in dynamic complex
environments (Endsley, 1995; Endsley, Roth,
352
Lerik, M., Hastjarjo, T. and Dharmastiti, R.
Situation Awareness Viewed from Sense of Direction and Choice of Navigation Direction.
DOI: 10.5220/0009820503520362
In Proceedings of the 1st International Conference on Psychology (ICPsy 2019), pages 352-362
ISBN: 978-989-758-448-0
Copyright
c
2020 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
Ridpath, & Neill, 2003). Although there are a
number of methods proposed to measure situation
awareness, one of the most popular is the Situation
Awareness Global Assessment Technique
(SAGAT), which involves simulations of "freezing"
and the submission of different types of questions
relating to current conditions and the near future. A
level 1 situation awareness check is proposed to
measure perceptions of elements of an environment
in a time and space framework, a level 2 situation
awareness check is proposed to measure an
integrated "meaning" understanding in the
environment, and a level 3 situation awareness
check is proposed to measure projections for the
near future then (Endsley, 1995; Endsley et al.,
2003). Measurement of this situation awareness is
generally applied for study conditions on dynamic
object tracking (eg Pylyshyn & Strom, 1988) and
static visual working memory (Luck & Vogel, 1997;
Vogel, Woodman, & Luck, 2001; Fougnie, Asplund,
& Marois, 2010).
Situation awareness measurements are designed
around the domain of special expertise, for example
in the air traffic controls officers, military
commands and other controlled tasks. Situation
awareness measurements are often designed to
evaluate technology as well as humans to determine
whether the system supports the user's ability to
understand different levels and situations. However,
situation awareness measurement can also be used to
measure individual achievement, as was done in this
research (Endsley et al., 2003). This makes this
research perhaps the first research that measures the
general awareness of an individual's situation in
terms of direction sensitivity, and choice of
directions in navigation.
This study is designed to test the situation
awareness measurement procedures with
hypotheses: (1) there are differences in situation
awareness in terms of sense of direction, (2) there
are differences in situation awareness in terms of
individual direction choices, (3) there are differences
in situation awareness in terms of gender.
2 RESEARCH METHOD
2.1 Participants
Participants were invited to the participants of basic
psychology courses of the psychology Faculty of a
State University in Yogyakarta totaling 165 people,
120 female and 45 male. Ages between 18-19 years
old.
2.2 Measurement
2.2.1 Situation Awareness Test
Situation awareness tests are found in The
Psychological Experiment Building Language Test
Battery Version 0.14 (Mueller, 2010). A complete
source of this situation awareness test can be
downloaded at http://pebl.sf.net, the version used in
this research is a version that has been translated into
Indonesian. This situation awareness test has been
used to measure the attention load caused by heat
stress and places of dynamic attention (Mueller,
Simpkins, Price, Weber, & McClellan, 2011), and
has been shown to detect cognitive weakness due to
these pressures. This situation awareness test has
also been used to detect cognitive weakness for
obsessive-compulsive patients (Tumkaya et al.
2013).
Situation awareness tests are designed to
measure the same basic factors as the Situation
Awareness Global Assessment Technique (Endsley
& Garland, 2000). This situation awareness test
takes the form of a dynamic visual footprint.
Participants were asked to monitor the location,
identity and movement of a set of target animal
images, consisting of 5 animals that move around a
quadrilateral area. The target animal image consists
of three insects (a fly, a spider and an ant) and two
lizards (one yellow and one blue-green). The motion
simulation is this: for each insect target, a
destination location is uniformly sampled in space,
and for each lizard, an insect target is determined.
For each circle of the simulation (screen display),
each target's is uniformly determined in space, and
for each lizard, a target is chosen. At each round of
the simulation (screen display), each target goal is
determined by uniform sampling from the direction
of 12.5 degrees on each side of the direction from
the target of the destination. Each target move with a
uniform distance from the target for its destination
(at a speed of 100pixels / s). This scheme gives the
impression of a biological movement that leads to
general goals but with some deviations.
Furthermore, each lizard moves towards an insect,
while each insect moves to the target location
without being influenced by other targets. Whenever
a predator lizard comes in a small threshold for a
target insect, (1) the insect is eaten, (2) a new
location and destination is chosen for the insect, and
(3) a new insect target is chosen for the lizard.
Between the dynamics of the simulation, the target
Situation Awareness Viewed from Sense of Direction and Choice of Navigation Direction
353
screen was changed and one interruption of the three
situation awareness interruptions was given.This
situation awareness test takes the form of a dynamic
visual footprint. Participants were asked to monitor
the location, identity and movement of a set of target
animal images, consisting of 5 animals that move
around a quadrilateral area. The target animal image
consists of three insects (a fly, a spider and an ant)
and two lizards (one yellow and one blue-green).
The motion simulation is like this: for each insect
target, a destination location is uniformly sampled in
space, and for each lizard, an insect target is
determined. For each circle in the simulation (screen
display), each target's target is uniformly determined
in space, and for each lizard, an insect target is
chosen. At each round of the simulation (screen
display), each target goal is determined by uniform
sampling from the direction of 12.5 degrees on each
side of the direction from the target to the
destination. Each target moves with a uniform
distance from the target to its destination (at a speed
of 100pixels / s). This scheme gives the impression
of a biological movement that leads to general goals
but with some deviations. Furthermore, each lizard
moves towards an insect, while each insect moves to
the target location without being influenced by other
targets. Whenever a predator lizard comes in a small
threshold for a target insect, (1) the insect is eaten,
(2) a new location and destination is chosen for the
insect, and (3) a new insect target is chosen for the
lizard. Between the dynamics of the simulation, the
target screen was changed and one interruption of
the three situation awareness interruptions was
given.
A level 2 situation awareness interrupts
participants to click on the location of the five
targets. A small red circle is placed at the location of
the click, and there is no chance of not clicking.
Level 2 situation awareness interruptions ask
participants to identify the type of target.
Interruptions in a location are given (which is the
actual location of one of the targets), and five target
images are displayed below the quadrilateral area.
Participants are asked to click on the target's identity
at the location of the interruption. Two interruptions
were given for each trial. This type of interruption
requires the integration of knowledge about the
target's identity with the location, which involves
concurrent attention to the animal that is chasing and
being chased after the screen.Finally, a level 3
situation awareness interrupts participants to identify
the direction of the head of a specific target that is
moving, and click on target spot on the screen to
position the animal's head direction. Participants are
free to adjust the direction of the target head by
clicking, after that click the button that says "ok".
Instructions are given on the screen for the test. The
following instructions are given before each block's
task and the interrupt picture for each block.
1. Exercise Block
Figure 1: Instructions.
Figure 2: Interruptions 1.
Figure 3: Instructions 2.
ICPsy 2019 - International Conference on Psychology
354
Figure 4: Interruptions 2.
Figure 5: Instructions 3.
Figure 3: Interruptions 3.
Test Block Instructions
Interrupt 1: now, you will do a long block of
the first type of interruption. In this trial, you
only need to monitor the location of the
animals. Click the mouse to start.
Interrupt 2: now, you will do a long block of
the second type of interruption. In this trial you
will identify the types of animals at the location
of the interruption. Click the mouse to start.
Interrupt 3: now, you will do a long block of
the third type of interruption. In this trial you
will identify the direction of the animal's head.
Click the mouse to start.
Mixed Block Instructions
Finally, you will work on a block where one of
the three interruptions will appear. Click the
mouse to start. Thank, you can proceed with
the next test.
2. Santa Barbara Sense of Direction Scale
This scales of 15 items a Likert scales adapted to
the self-report scale of environmental spatial
ability (Hegarty et al., 2002). Each item is a self-
report statement from several aspects of
environmental spatial cognition; participants
responded by circling numbers from 1 (strongly
agree) to 7 (strongly disagree). These scale items
contain half positive statements and half negative
statements. One example of a positive item is "I
am very good at assessing distance"; and an
example of a negative item is "I have trouble
with direction". Positive item scoring is reversed
and the highest score indicates better sense of
direction. The total number of 15 item scores
were used for analysis. Internal reliability of this
test is 0.88.
The directions selection questionnaire contains
one item with a statement that says " When I get
lost in a new location, I will look for directions
that are compelling":
Map : never 1 2 3 4 5 6
7 Always
Verbal instructions : never 1 2 3 4 5 6
7 Always
The choice of directions is determined based
on the choice of the highest number scoring
between maps or verbal instructions. If the
subject circles the same number, the choice of
subject directions is called both map and verbal
instructions.
Procedure
Participants are invited to participate in
research in basic psychology classes.
Participants who register then are given a
schedule to follow the implementation of data
collection of a computer laboratory. First the
participants fill out and sign informed consent.
After that the participants carried out the
situation awareness task contained in The
Psychology Experiment Building Language
(PEBL) Test Battery software, version 0.14
(Mueler, 2010). Then the participants filled out
the Santa Barbara Sense of Direction scale that
had been adapted to Indonesian and the
directions selection questionnaire.
Researchers see hierarchy of situation
awareness as a way to examine different types
of information about a situation. In our
Situation Awareness Viewed from Sense of Direction and Choice of Navigation Direction
355
implementation, level 1 situation awareness
interruptions only recover location objects;
situation 2 level awareness interruptions
provide a location (and do not involve retrieval
of information) and only require a
reassignment. Level 3 situation awareness
interruptions do not ask for identity and
position, only ask for the direction of the target
animal's head.
The process of situation awareness task
performance takes place through a number of
blocks. First, three short training blocks are
given; one block for each level of situation
awareness interruption. Then, three pure block
tests are given, one block test for each situation
awareness level (sequential from levels 1-3),
with 15 trials per block. Finally, one mixed
block was given, containing 15 trials from each
interruption, for a total of 45 trials in the block.
In the mixed block, participants are not aware
of the next level of situation interruption
awareness requested (Level 1, level 2 or level
3). Thus, data onto mixed blocks used in
analysis and participants are free to direct
attention compared to pure block tests where
participants' attention is only directed towards
one goal (Endsley et al., 2003). The situation
awareness performance lasts 22.5 minutes.
Participants are permitted to rest between
blocks
Analysis
Data were analyzed using SPSS, version 15.0
for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).
Level 1, level 2, and level 3 situation
awareness scores were compared to the group
average with categorized direction sensitivity
scores, choice of directions and gender using
ONE-WAY ANOVA or also called one-way
variant analysis.
3 RESULT
The results of the analysis of research data onto the
form of a statistical summary of the three choices of
directions in navigation are listed in table 1 below.
For example, a description of level 1 situation
awareness, participants who choose maps as
directions are a mean of 0.4879 with a minimum of
0.30 and a maximum of 0.87. Level of confidence
95% or a significance of 5%, the mean awareness of
the situation are at 0.4594 to 0.5163. And so on.
Table 1: The Situation Awareness Description is Viewed
from the Choice of Directions (Descriptive).
The assumption test results of ANOVA in table 2
show that the results of the Levene Test calculate
situation awareness levels 1.2, and 3 with a
probability of 0.338; 0.570; 0.121> 0.05, then all
three variances is the same. The assumption of
variance similarity with the ANOVA Test has been
fulfilled.
Table 2: Test the Situation Awareness Data Assumptions
Based on the Choice of Directions.
Levene
statistic
Df 1 Df2 Sig.
Situation
awareness level 1
1,091 2 161 0,338
Situation
awareness level 2
0,563 2 161 0,570
Situation
awareness level 3
2,141 2 161 0,121
The results of the analysis of the variance in the
one-way situation awareness in terms of the choice
of directions are shown in table 3 below. Level 1
situation awareness, based on F arithmetic 3,743,
degrees of freedom 3 and a significance level of 5%,
the F values in the table obtained 2.66. F counts
3.743 with a probability of 0.012 <0.05. The
conclusion of the mean level 1 situation awareness
in the three groups of direction choices is indeed
different. Level 2 situation awareness with an F
counts1.952, a degree of freedom 3 and a
significance level of 5%, the F values in the table
obtained 2.66. F counts 1.952 > F table 2.66. So, F
counts with a probability of 0.123> 0.05. Conclusion
level 2 situation awareness in the three groups of
direction choices there is no difference. Situation
awareness level 3, based on F counts 4,640, degrees
of freedom 3 and a significance level of 5%, the F
59 .4879 .10918 .01421 .4594 .5163 .30 .87
62 .5394 .11763 .01494 .5095 .5693 .30 .80
43 .4762 .12092 .01844 .4389 .5134 .28 .76
1 .3500 . . . . .35 .35
165 .5033 .11841 .00922 .4851 .5215 .28 .87
59 .3175 .10820 .01409 .2893 .3457 .13 .63
62 .2957 .12679 .01610 .2635 .3279 .03 .63
43 .3481 .12179 .01857 .3106 .3856 .10 .57
1 .4330 . . . . .43 .43
165 .3180 .12010 .00935 .2995 .3364 .03 .63
59 66.7119 17.64208 2.29680 62.1143 71.3094 34.00 119.00
62 76.8710 17.07888 2.16902 72.5337 81.2082 33.00 110.00
43 65.3023 21.05383 3.21068 58.8229 71.7817 31.00 116.00
1 56.0000 . . . . 56.00 56.00
165 70.0970 18.99462 1.47873 67.1772 73.0168 31.00 119.00
peta
petunjuk verbal
Peta dan Petunju
k
6
Total
peta
petunjuk verbal
Peta dan Petunju
k
6
Total
peta
petunjuk verbal
Peta dan Petunju
k
6
Total
Kesadaran
S
Kesadaran s
i
Kesadaran s
i
N Mean
t
d. DeviatioStd. Error
L
ower Bou
n
U
pper Bou
n
%
Confidence Interval
Mean
Minimum
M
aximu
m
ICPsy 2019 - International Conference on Psychology
356
values in the table obtained 2.66. F counts 4.640
with a probability of 0.04 <0.05. In conclusion there
is a difference in the mean level 3 situation
awareness in the three groups of direction choices.
Table 3: ANOVA.
Sum of
s
q
uares
df Mean
s
uare
F Sig.
Kesa
dara
n
situa
si
Betwee
n
groups
.150 3 .050 37
43
0.1
2
Within
g
rou
p
s
2.150 16
1
.013
Total 2.300 16
4
Kesa
dara
n
situa
si
Betwee
n
groups
0.83 3 0.28 19
52
.12
3
Within
groups
2.283 16
1
0.14
Total 2.366 16
4
Kesa
dara
n
situa
si
Betwee
n
groups
4708.30
9
3 1569.
436
46
40
.00
4
Within
g
rou
p
s
54462.1
39
16
1
338.2
74
Total 59170.4
48
16
4
T tests results from comparing the choice of
higher directions in level 1 awareness, in table 4
below. In the group that choose the map, the mean
0.4879 and the group that chose the verbal
instructions, the mean 0.5394. The results of one-
tailed t test for independent samples of table 5, seen
0.014 / 2 = 0.007 <0.005, there is a significant
difference, the choice of map directions higher than
verbal instruction.
Table 4: Group Statistical T-Test.
Petunjuk
arah
N Mean Std.
deviati
on
Std.
Error
Mean
Kesad
aran
situasi
Peta 59 .4879 .1091
8
.01421
Petunjuk
verbal
62 .5394 .1176
3
.01494
Table 5: Independent Sample T-Test between Map and
Verbal Instruction.
T-test results to compare the higher direction
choices of level 1 situation awareness, in table 6
below. In the group that chose verbal instructions,
mean 0.5394 and groups that chose map and verbal
instructions, mean 0.4762. The results of the one-
tailed t test of independent samples of table 7, seen
0.009 / 2 = 0.0045 < 0.005, there is a significant
difference between the choice of map directions and
verbal instructions. Verbal instructions mean 0.5394
are higher than map directions and verbal Instruction
mean of 0.4762.
Table 6: Group Statistical T-Test.
Petunjuk
arah
N Mean Std.
deviati
on
Std.
Error
Mean
Kesad
aran
situasi
Petunjuk
verbal
62 .5394 .1176
3
.01494
Peta dan
petunjuk
verbal
43 .4762 .1209
2
.01844
Table 7: Independent Samples Test.
T-test results to compare the higher direction
choices of level 1 situation awareness, in table 7
below. In the group that chooses map directions, the
mean is 0.4879 and the group that chooses map
instructions and is verbal, the mean is 0.4762. The
results of the one-tailed t test of independent
samples of table 8, seen 0.610 / 2 = 0.305 > 0.005,
there is no difference between the choice of map
directions and map directions and verbal.
Situation Awareness Viewed from Sense of Direction and Choice of Navigation Direction
357
Table 8: Group Statistical T-Test.
Petunjuk
arah
N Mean Std.
deviati
on
Std.
Error
Mean
Kesad
aran
situasi
Petunjuk
verbal
59 .4879 .1091
8
.01421
Peta dan
petunjuk
verbal
43 .4762 .1209
2
.01844
Table 9: Independent Samples Test.
T tests results from comparing the choice of
directions higher in level 3 awareness, in table 10
below. In the group that chose maps, the mean
66.7119 and the group that chose verbal cues, mean
76.8710. The results of one-tailed t test for
independent samples of table 11, seen 0.001 < 0.005,
map directions higher than verbal instruction.
Table 10: Group Statistical T-Test.
Petunjuk
arah
N Mean Std.
deviati
on
Std.
Error
Mean
Kesad
aran
situasi
Peta 59 66.71
19
17.64
208
2.2968
0
Petunjuk
verbal
43 76.87
10
17.08
888
2.1690
2
Table 11: Independent Samples Test.
T tests results from comparing the choice of
directions higher in level 3 situation awareness, in
table 12 below. In the group that chose verbal
instructions, mean 76.8710 and group that chose
verbal and map directions, mean 65.3023. The
results of one-tailed t test for independent samples of
table 13, seen 0.002 / 2 = 0.001 < 0.005, there is a
significant differences, verbal instruction higher than
map direction.
Table 12: Group Statistical T-Test.
Petunjuk
arah
N Mean Std.
deviati
on
Std.
Error
Mean
Kesad
aran
situasi
Petunjuk
verbal
62 76.87
10
17.07
888
2.1690
2
Peta dan
petunjuk
verbal
43 65.30
23
21.05
383
3.2106
8
Table 13: Independent Samples Test.
T tests results from comparing the choice of
directions higher in level 3 situation awareness, in
table 14 below. In the group that chose map
directions, mean 66.7119 and groups that chose map
and verbal directions, mean 65.3023. The results of
the one-tailed t test of independent samples of table
15, seen 0.714 / 2 = 0.357 > 0.005, there was no
significant difference between the choices of verbal
instruction and map and verbal instruction.
Table 14: Group Statistical T-Test.
Petunjuk
arah
N Mean Std.
deviati
on
Std.
Error
Mean
Kesad
aran
situasi
Peta 59 66.71
19
17.64
208
2.2968
0
Peta dan
petunjuk
verbal
43 76.87
10
17.08
888
2.1690
2
Table 15: Independent Samples Test.
2.181 .143 .511 100 .610 .01172
0
2291
0
3373
0
5717
.503 5.009 .616 .01172
0
2328
0
3457
0
5801
Equal varia
n
Equal varia
n
Kesada
r
F Sig.
e
's Test for E
q
of Variances
t df . (2-tai
l
a
n Differ
e
t
d. Err
o
i
fferen
c
LowerUpper
o
nfidence Int
e
t
he Differenc
e
t-test for Equality of Means
.018 .893 .218 119 .002 15910
5
655
0
938
0
883
.216 .197 .002 15910
5
911
1
489
0
332
Equal var
i
Equal var
i
Kesa
d
F Sig.
s
Test for
E
f Varianc
e
t df (2-ta
i
n
Diffe
r
d
. Er
r
f
fere
n
L
owe
r
U
ppe
r
n
fidence I
n
e
Differen
c
t-test for Equality of Means
3.676 .058 3.100 103 .002
1
.56864
7
3127
1
6855
9
6873
2.986
7
7.911 .004
1
.56864
8
7467
8
5462
2
8266
Equal varian
c
Equal varian
c
Kesada
r
F Sig.
e
's Test for E
q
of Variances
t df
g
. (2-tail
a
n Differ
e
t
d. Err
o
i
fferen
c
Lower Upper
o
nfidence Int
e
t
he Differenc
e
t-test for Equality of Means
2.961 .088 .367 100 .714 1.40954
3
.83964
6
.20820
9
.02727
.357 80.685 .722 1.40954
3
.94763
6
.44547
9
.26454
Equal variances
Equal variances
Kesadara
n
F Sig.
e
ne's Test for Eq
u
of Variances
t df
g
. (2-tail
e
e
an Differe
n
S
td. Erro
r
D
ifferenc
e
Lower Upper
C
onfidence Inter
v
the Difference
t-test for Equality of Means
ICPsy 2019 - International Conference on Psychology
358
The results of the analysis of research data onto
the form of a summary of direction sensitivity
statistics in navigation are in table 16 below.
Suppose a description of level 1 situation awareness,
participants who have a good sense of direction are
the mean 0.4939 with a minimum of 0.30 and a
maximum of 0.87. Level of confidence 95% or a
significance of 5%, situation awareness at 0.4406 to
0.5472 and so on.
Table 16: Descriptive.
The assumption test results of ANOVA in table
17 show that the Lavene Test results to calculate
situation awareness levels 1,2, and 3 with a
probability of 0.566; 0.961; 2,012 > 0.05 then all
three variances are the same. So the assumption of
variance similarity the ANOVA Test has been
fulfilled.
Table 17: Assumptions Data Test.
Lavene
statisti
k
Df
1
Df2 Sig.
Situation
awareness Level
1
0,566 2 161 .569
Situation
Awareness Level
2
0,961 2 161 .385
Situation
awareness Level
3
2,012 2 161 .137
The results of the analysis of the variance
between one-way situation awareness in sense of
direction are shown in table 18 below. Level 1
situation awareness, based on an F count of 0.106, a
degree of freedom 2 and a significance level of 5%,
the F values of the table is 3.05. F counts 0.106 < F
table 3.05 with a probability of 0.899 > 0.05. The
conclusion of the mean situation awareness level 1
in the three groups of direction sensitivity was no
difference. Situation awareness level 2 with an F
counts of 0.179, a degree of freedom 2 and a
significance level of 5%, the F values in the table is
3.05. So, F counts with a probability of 0.836> 0.05.
Conclusion situation awareness level 2 in the three
groups sense of direction there is no difference.
Situation awareness level 3, based on an F counts of
0.690, a degree of freedom and level of significance
5%, the F values in the table is 3.05. So, F counts
0.690 < F table 3.05 with probability 0.503 > 0.05.
The conclusion of the mean Situation awareness
level 3 in the three groups of sense of direction no
difference.
Table 18: One Way Anova.
Sum of
s
q
uares
df Mean
s
uare
F Sig.
Kesa
dara
n
situa
si
Betwee
n
groups
.003 2 .002 .1
06
.89
9
Within
g
rou
p
s
2.297 16
2
.014
Total 2.300 16
4
Kesa
dara
n
situa
si
Betwee
n
groups
.005 2 .003 .1
79
.83
6
Within
g
rou
p
s
2.360 16
2
.015
Total 2.366 16
4
Kesa
dara
n
situa
si
Betwee
n
groups
499.795 2 249.8
98
.6
90
.50
3
Within
groups
58670.6
53
16
2
362.1
65
Total 59170.4
48
16
4
The results of the analysis of research data onto
the form of a statistical summary of the sexes are in
table 19 below. Description of situation 1 level
awareness, female participants with a mean of
0.5094 with a minimum of 0.32 and a maximum of
0.80. With a confidence level of 95% or a
significance of 5%, mean the situation awareness
level 1 female participants were at 0.4888 to 0.5301.
23 .4939 .12319 .02569 .4406 .5472 .30 .87
122 .5057 .12049 .01091 .4841 .5273 .28 .80
20 .4997 .10410 .02328 .4509 .5484 .34 .78
165 .5033 .11841 .00922 .4851 .5215 .28 .87
23 .3086 .13459 .02806 .2504 .3668 .13 .60
122 .3213 .11459 .01037 .3008 .3418 .03 .63
20 .3083 .14011 .03133 .2428 .3739 .10 .60
165 .3180 .12010 .00935 .2995 .3364 .03 .63
23
6
7.9130
1
7.19385
3
.58517 60.4779 75.3482 34.00 119.00
122
6
9.7951
1
9.66410
1
.78030 66.2705 73.3197 31.00 116.00
20
7
4.4500
1
6.82565
3
.76233 66.5754 82.3246 38.00 103.00
165
7
0.0970
1
8.99462
1
.47873 67.1772 73.0168 31.00 119.00
baik
seda
n
b
uru
k
Tota
l
baik
seda
n
b
uru
k
Tota
l
baik
seda
n
b
uru
k
Tota
l
Kesadaran
Kesadaran
Kesadaran
N Mean d. Deviati
o
S
td. Erro
r
o
wer Bou
n
p
per Bou
n
Confidence Interv
a
Mean
M
inimu
m
M
aximu
m
Situation Awareness Viewed from Sense of Direction and Choice of Navigation Direction
359
Description of Situation awareness level 1, male
participants with a mean of 0.4871 with a minimum
of 0.28 and a maximum of 0.87. Level of
confidence 95% or a significance of 5%, the mean
situation awareness level 1 male participants were
at 0.4484 to 0.5259.
Table 19: Descriptive.
The assumption test results from ANOVA in
table 20 show that the Levene Test results to
calculate situation awareness levels 1,2, and 3 with a
probability of 0.299; 0.500; 0.242 > 0.05 then both
variances are the same. So the assumption of
variance similarity in the ANOVA Test has been
fulfilled.
Table 20: Assumption Test.
Levene
statistic
Df 1 Df2 Sig.
Situation
awareness level 1
1,084 1 163 0,299
Situation
awareness level 2
0,457 1 163 0,500
Situation
awareness level 3
1,376 1 163 0,242
The results of the analysis of variance in one
path of situation awareness by sex in table 21 below.
Situation awareness level 1, based on the F counts
1.161, degrees of freedom 1 and a significance level
of 5%, the F values in the table obtained the number
3.90. F calculates 1.161> F table 3.90 with a
probability of 0.283 > 0.05. The conclusion of the
mean level 1 situation awareness in the two sexes is
no difference. Level 2 situation awareness with an F
count of 0,000, a degree of freedom 1 and a
significance level of 5%, the F value in the table is
3.90. F calculate with a probability of 0.989 > 0.05.
Conclusion situation awareness level 2 in the two
sexes there is no difference. Situation awareness
level 3 on F counts of 9,365, degrees of freedom 1
and a level of 5%, the F value of the table is 3.90. F
calculated 9,365 > F table 3.90 with a probability of
0.003 < 0.05. There are significant differences in
Situation awareness level 3 between the sexes. This
difference is supported by the data onto table 19, the
mean of women 72.8000 is higher than the mean of
men 62.8889.
Table 21: One Way Anova.
4 DISCUSSION
The situation awareness tasks to involve
interruptions in the middle of a screen for the
computer screen, where participants are asked to
recall or reproduce different types of information.
Situation awareness level 1 interruptions only
require identification of the target location (ignoring
identity), this is interpreted by the author as an initial
perception of the sense media. Situation awareness
level2 interruptions require identification of targets
of specific locations, this is interpreted by the
authors as integrating and understanding
information, pattern recognition and the ability to
distinguish relevant and irrelevant information
(Soliman, 2010; Wright, Taekman, & Endsley,
2004). Interference level 3 situation awareness is
interpreted by the authors as anticipation and
projection of conditions in the near future then based
on current condition information to make decisions
and actions. Furthermore, situation awareness tasks
include one task test blocks where only one
information is the focus and multiple task test
blocks, where level 1, level 2 and level 3 situation
awareness information needs to be managed during
the trials (this data is analyzed).
The results of the analysis of situation awareness
in terms of directions provide evidence of
differences in situation awareness level 1 and level
3. Situation awareness level 1 as a level of
perception is related to landmark knowledge that
becomes a stimulus from the environment for
individuals to identify the location of a building,
120 .5094 .11418 .01042 .4888 .5301 .32 .80
45 .4871 .12898 .01923 .4484 .5259 .28 .87
165 .5033 .11841 .00922 .4851 .5215 .28 .87
120 .3180 .12113 .01106 .2961 .3399 .03 .63
45 .3178 .11868 .01769 .2821 .3534 .03 .60
165 .3180 .12010 .00935 .2995 .3364 .03 .63
120
7
2.8000
1
8.02594
1
.64554 69.5417 76.0583 33.00 119.00
45
6
2.8889
1
9.82219
2
.95492 56.9336 68.8441 31.00 109.00
165
7
0.0970
1
8.99462
1
.47873 67.1772 73.0168 31.00 119.00
p
erem
p
laki-la
k
Total
p
erem
p
laki-la
k
Total
p
erem
p
laki-la
k
Total
Kesadaran
Kesadaran
Kesadaran
N Mean
d
. Deviati
o
S
td. Erro
r
o
wer Bou
n
p
per Bou
n
Confidence Interv
a
Mean
M
inimu
m
M
aximu
m
.016 1 .016 1.161 .283
2.283 163 .014
2.300 164
.000 1 .000 .000 .989
2.366 163 .015
2.366 164
3214.804 1 3214.804 9.365 .003
55955.644 163 343.286
59170.448 164
Between Group
Within Groups
Total
Between Group
Within Groups
Total
Between Group
Within Groups
Total
Kesadaran Situa
s
Kesadaran situa
s
Kesadaran situa
s
Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig.
ICPsy 2019 - International Conference on Psychology
360
signs or other environmental property. This level of
perception of situation awareness is influenced by
the choice of individuals that prefer to use visual
directions also in the form of a map, verbal
instructions and both choices (Brunye & Taylor,
2008; Meilinger & Knauff, 2008; Lerik, Hastjarjo, &
Dharmastiti, 2016 ). These three choices of
directions when compared were found to be the
highest choice of verbal instructions, this is
supported by the results of the study of Brunye &
Taylor (2008) which states that when individuals get
verbal instructions to follow a route to a location,
verbal instructions form mental spatial models or
commonly known as cognitive maps.
Situation awareness level 3 as a level of
anticipation or prediction of conditions in the near
future then also gets influence from directions that
utilize the choice of map directions, verbal cues and
both. This is supported by Hirtle, et al. (2010) that
states situation awareness as spatial awareness is
part of survey knowledge that provides opportunities
for individuals to plan new routes, shortcuts, and
detours. Situation awareness level 2 which is an
integration of the environmental situation was found
not to be influenced by the choice of map directions,
verbal instructions or both. The cause of this
situation is not yet known, further research needs to
be done.
The analysis shows that situation awareness is
not influenced by individual sense of direction. This
may occur because self-sensitivity reports as
individual potential are associated with the
performance of navigation tasks in previous research
(Hegarty, Montello, Richardson, Ishikawa, &
Lovelace, 2006; Hund & Nazarczuk, 2009; Labate,
Pazzaglia, & Hegarty, 2014 ). In contrast to this
research that links the sense of direction witth the
situation awareness virtually. Therefore, further
research is needed.
Situation awareness level 3 based on gender
found differences. These results are consistent with
the situation awareness theory which states that
situation awareness is a hierarchical level where
level 3 includes level 2 and level 1, and level 2
includes level 1 (Endsley, 1995).
5 CONCLUSION
The choice of directions in the form of maps, verbal
instructions and choice of map directions and verbal
instructions when navigation affects individual
differences in situation awareness at level 1 and
level 3. The choice of directions turns out to be
found no difference in individual level 2 situation
awareness. There was no apparent difference in
direction sensitivity in individuals in situation 1, 2 or
3 level awareness. Gender was significantly different
in level 3 situation awareness, women were higher
than men.
Further research that looks at sense of direction
and choice of directions is very important to do by
linking the scale of self-reports to the performance
of behaviors in the real environment. Experimental
research is needed to measure situation awareness
variables related to route knowledge in an effort to
find a location.
REFERENCES
Brunyé, T. T., & Taylor, H. A. (2008). Extended
experience benefits spatial mental model development
with route but not survey descriptions. Acta
psychologica, 127(2), 340-354.
Endsley, M. R. (1988, May). Situation awareness global
assessment technique (SAGAT). In Aerospace and
Electronics Conference, 1988. NAECON 1988.,
Proceedings of the IEEE 1988 National (pp. 789-795).
IEEE.
Endsley, M. R. (1995). Toward a theory of situation
awareness in dynamic systems. Human Factors: The
Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics
Society, 37(1), 32-64.
Endsley, M. R., & Garland, D. J. (2000). Theoretical
underpinnings of situation awareness: A critical
review. Situation awareness analysis and
measurement, 3-32.
Endsley, J. J., Roth, J. A., Ridpath, J., & Neill, J. (2003).
Maternal antibody blocks humoral but not T cell
responses to BVDV. Biologicals, 31(2), 123-125.
Fougnie, D., Asplund, C. L., & Marois, R. (2010). What
are the units of storage in visual working memory?.
Journal of vision, 10(12), 27-27.
Giudice, N. A., Bakdash, J. Z., & Legge, G. E. (2007).
Wayfinding with words: spatial learning and
navigation using dynamically updated verbal
descriptions. Psychological research, 71(3), 347-358.
Hegarty, M., Montello, D.R., Richardson,A.E.,
Lovelace,K., & Subhiah, I. (2002). Development of a
self-report measure of environmental spatial ability.
Inteligence,30,425-447.
Hegarty, M., Montello, D. R., Richardson, A. E.,
Ishikawa, T., & Lovelace, K. (2006). Spatial abilities
at different scales: Individual differences in aptitude-
test performance and spatial-layout learning.
Intelligence, 34(2), 151-176.
Hund, A. M., & Nazarczuk, S. N. (2009). The effects of
sense of direction and training experience on
wayfinding efficiency. Journal of Environmental
Psychology, 29(1), 151-159.
Situation Awareness Viewed from Sense of Direction and Choice of Navigation Direction
361
Hund, A.M., & Padgitt, A.J. (2010). Direction giving and
following in the service of wayfinding in a complex
indoor environment. Journal of Environmental
Psychology, 30,553-564.
Ishikawa, T., & Montello, D. R. (2006). Spatial
knowledge acquisition from direct experience in the
environment: Individual differences in the
development of metric knowledge and the integ-ration
of separately learned places. Cognitive Psychology,
52, 93–129.
Kato, Y., & Takeuchi, Y. (2003). Individual differences in
wayfinding strategies. Journal of Environmental
Psychology, 23(2), 171-188.
Klippel, A., Hirtle, S., & Davies, C. (2010). You-are-here
maps: Creating spatial awareness through map-like
representations. Spatial Cognition & Computation,
10(2-3), 83-93.
Kozlowski,L.T.,& Bryant,K.J.(1977). Sense of
Direction,spatial orientation, and cognitif maps.
Journal of Experimental Psychology : Human
Perseption and Performance,3(4), 590-598.
Labate, E., Pazzaglia, F., & Hegarty, M. (2014). What
working memory subcomponents are needed in the
acquisition of survey knowledge? Evidence from
direction estimation and shortcut tasks. Journal of
Environmental Psychology, 37, 73-79.
Lerik,M.D.C., Hastjarjo,T.D.,& Dharmastiti,Rini.
(2016,April). Mencari lokasi baru: meminta petunjuk
verbal atau peta (studi pendahuluan). Proceeding
seminar nasional mesin dan industri (SNMI-X) 2016.
Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual
working memory for features and conjunctions.
Nature, 390(6657), 279-281.
Meilinger, T.,& Knauff,M. (2008). Ask for direction or
use a map : A field experiment on spatial orientation
and wayfinding in an urban environment. Journal of
Spatial Science, 53 (2), 13-24.
Montello, D.R., & Pick,H.L.(1993). Integrating
knowledge of vertically aligned large-scale space.
Environment and Behaviour , 25, 457-484.
Mueller, S.T.(2010). The Psychology Experiment
Building Language,Version 0,14.Software
downloaded from (http://pebl.sourceforge.net).
Mueller, S. T., Simpkins, B., Price, O. T., Weber, P., &
McClellan, G. E. (2011). Cognitive performance
degradation with the T3 methodology (pp. 579-588).
Final Technical Report. Applied Research Associates
Inc., Arlington.
Pylyshyn, Z. W., & Storm, R. W. (1988). Tracking
multiple independent targets: Evidence for a parallel
tracking mechanism. Spatial vision, 3(3), 179-197.
Sholl, M.J., Kenny,R.J., & DellaPorta,K.A., (2006).
Allocentric-heading recall and its relation to self-
reported sense-of-direction. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 32
(3),516-533.
Soliman,A.M. (2010). Exploring the central executive in
situation awareness. Psychological Report, 106, 105-
118.
Tumkaya, S., Karadag, F., Mueller, S. T., Ugurlu, T. T.,
Oguzhanoglu, N. K., Ozdel, O., & Bayraktutan, M.
(2013). Situation awareness in obsessive-compulsive
disorder. Psychiatry research, 209(3), 579-588.
Vogel, E. K., Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (2001).
Storage of features, conjunctions, and objects in visual
working memory. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,
27(1), 92.
Wright, M.C., Taekman, J.M., & Endsley,M.R., (2004).
Objective measures of situation awareness in a
simulated medical environment. Quality and Safety in
Health Care Suppl,1 , 565-571.
ICPsy 2019 - International Conference on Psychology
362