Analysing the Use of Worked Examples and Tutored and Untutored
Problem-Solving in a Dispositional Learning Analytics Context
Dirk T. Tempelaar
1
, Bart Rienties
2
and Quan Nguyen
2
1
Maastricht University, School of Business and Economics, PO Box 616, 6200 MD Maastricht, The Netherlands
2
Open University U.K., Institute of Educational Technology, Walton Hal, Milton Keynes, MK7 6AA, U.K.
Keywords: Blended Learning, Dispositional Learning Analytics, Learning Strategies, Multi-modal Data, Prediction
Models, Tutored Problem-Solving, Untutored Problem-Solving, Worked Examples.
Abstract: The identification of students’ learning strategies by using multi-modal data that combine trace data with
self-report data is the prime aim of this study. Our context is an application of dispositional learning
analytics in a large introductory course mathematics and statistics, based on blended learning. Building on
previous studies in which we found marked differences in how students use worked examples as a learning
strategy, we compare different profiles of learning strategies on learning dispositions and learning outcome.
Our results cast a new light on the issue of efficiency of learning by worked examples, tutored and untutored
problem-solving: in contexts where students can apply their own preferred learning strategy, we find that
learning strategies depend on learning dispositions. As a result, learning dispositions will have a
confounding effect when studying the efficiency of worked examples as a learning strategy in an
ecologically valid context.
1 INTRODUCTION
For many decades, research into student learning
tactics and strategies has primarily relied on self-
reports or think-aloud protocols, open to the bias
often present in self-reported perceptions, or
excluding naturalistic contexts from the analysis
(Azevedo, Harley, Trevors, Duffy, Feyzi-Behnagh,
Bouchet, et al., 2013; Gašević, Jovanović, Pardo, &
Dawson, 2017; Gašević, Mirriahi, Dawson, &
Joksimović, 2017). The increasing use of blended
learning and other forms of technology-enhanced
education gave way to measure revealed learning
strategies by collecting traces of students’ learning
behaviours in the digital learning platforms. This
new opportunity of combining trace data with self-
report data has boosted empirical research in
learning tactics and strategies. Examples of such are
Azevedo et al. (2013), and research by Gašević and
co-authors (Gašević, Jovanović, et al. 2017;
Gašević, Mirriahi, et al., 2017).
This type of research aims to investigate
relationships between learning strategies measured
by trace data, learning approaches measured by self-
reports, and academic performance as learning
outcomes. For instance, Gašević, Jovanović et al.
(2017) finds that learning strategies are related to
deep learning approaches, but not to surface learning
approaches. In the experimental study Gašević,
Mirriahi, et al. (2017), the role of instructional
conditions and prior experience with technology-
enhanced education is investigated. However, most
of these studies do not take individual differences
into account, as expressed in Gašević, Mirriahi, et al.
(2017, p. 216): ‘Future studies should also account
for the effects of individual differences -e.g.,
motivation to use technology, self-efcacy about the
subject matter and/or technology, achievement goal
orientation, approaches to learning, and
metacognitive awareness’.
Our paper aims to contribute to this lack of
empirical work incorporating individual differences,
by addressing students’ learning strategies within a
dispositional learning analytics context. The
Dispositional Learning Analytics (DLA)
infrastructure, introduced by Buckingham Shum and
Crick (2012), combines learning data (generated in
learning activities through technology-enhanced
systems) with learner data (student dispositions,
values, and attitudes measured through self-report
surveys). Learning dispositions represent individual
difference characteristics that impact all learning
294
Tempelaar, D., Rienties, B. and Nguyen, Q.
Analysing the Use of Worked Examples and Tutored and Untutored Problem-Solving in a Dispositional Learning Analytics Context.
DOI: 10.5220/0006760202940301
In Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Computer Supported Education (CSEDU 2018), pages 294-301
ISBN: 978-989-758-291-2
Copyright
c
2019 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
processes and include affective, behavioural and
cognitive facets (Rienties, Cross, & Zdrahal, 2017).
Student’s preferred learning approaches are
examples of such dispositions of both cognitive and
behavioural type.
The current study builds on our previous DLA-
based research (Nguyen, Tempelaar, Rienties, &
Giesbers, 2016; Tempelaar, Cuypers, Van de Vrie,
Heck, & Van der Kooij, 2013; Tempelaar,
Mittelmeier, Rienties, & Nguyen, 2017; Tempelaar,
Rienties, & Giesbers, 2015; Tempelaar, Rienties, &
Nguyen, 2017). One of our empirical findings in
these studies was that traces of student learning in
digital platforms show marked differences in the use
of worked examples (Nguyen et al., 2016;
Tempelaar, Mittelmeier, et al., 2017; Tempelaar,
Rienties, & Nguyen, 2017). The merits of the
worked examples principle Renkl (2014) in the
initial acquisition of cognitive skills are well
documented. The use of worked solutions in multi-
media based learning environments stimulates
gaining deep understanding (Renkl, 2014). When
compared to the use of erroneous examples, tutored
problem-solving, and problem-solving in computer-
based environments, the use of worked examples
may be more efficient as it reaches similar learning
outcomes in less time and with less learning efforts.
The mechanism responsible for this outcome is
disclosed in Renkl (2014, p. 400): ‘examples relieve
learners of problem-solving that – in initial cognitive
skill acquisition when learners still lack
understanding – is typically slow, error-prone, and
driven by superficial strategies. When beginning
learners solve problems, the corresponding demands
may burden working memory capacities or even
overload them, which strengthens learners’ surface
orientation. … When learning from examples,
learners have enough working memory capacity for
self-explaining and comparing examples by which
abstract principles can be considered, and those
principles are then related to concrete exemplars. In
this way, learners gain an understanding of how to
apply principles in problem-solving and how to
relate problem cases to underlying principles’.
Following research by McLaren and co-authors
(McLaren, van Gog, Ganoe, Karabinos, & Yaron,
2016; McLaren, van Gog, Ganoe, Yaron, &
Karabinos, 2014), we extend the range of preferred
learning strategies taken into account to include,
beyond worked-examples, the tutored and untutored
problem-solving strategies. In the tutored problem-
solving strategy, students receive feedback in the
form of hints and an evaluation of provided answers,
both during and at the end of the problem-solving
steps. In untutored problem-solving, feedback is
restricted to the evaluation of provided answers at
the end of the problem-solving steps (McLaren et
al., 2014, 2016).
Evidence for the worked examples principle is
typically based on laboratory-based experimental
studies, in which the effectiveness of different
instructional designs is compared (Renkl, 2014).
McLaren and co-authors take the research into the
effectiveness of several learning strategies a step
into the direction of ecological validity, by choosing
for an experimental design in a classroom context,
assigning the alternative learning approaches
worked-examples, tutored and untutored problem-
solving, and erroneous examples as the conditions of
the experiment (McLaren et al., 2014, 2016). In our
research, we increase ecological validity one more
step by offering a digital learning environment that
encompasses all learning strategies of worked-
examples, tutored and untutored problem-solving,
and observing the revealed preference of the
students in terms of learning strategy they apply. In
this naturalistic context, the potential contribution of
LA-based investigations is that we can observe
students’ revealed preferences for a specific learning
strategy, how these preferences depend on the
learning task at hand, and how these preferences link
to other observations, such an individual difference
characteristics. By doing so, we aim to derive a
characterization of students who actively apply
worked examples or tutored problem-solving, and
those not doing so. In line with contemporary
research into learning strategies applying trace data
(Gašević, Jovanović, et al. 2017; Gašević, Mirriahi,
et al., 2017), we adopt two research questions: 1)
how does the choice for learning strategy relate to
learning dispositions? and 2) how does the learning
strategy of using worked examples or tutored
problem-solving relate to learning outcomes?
2 METHODS
2.1 Context of the Empirical Study
This study takes place in a large-scale introductory
mathematics and statistics course for first-year
undergraduate students in a business and economics
programme in the Netherlands. The educational
system is best described as ‘blended’ or ‘hybrid’.
The main component is face-to-face: Problem-Based
Learning (PBL), in small groups (14 students),
coached by a content expert tutor. Participation in
tutorial groups is required. Optional is the online
Analysing the Use of Worked Examples and Tutored and Untutored Problem-Solving in a Dispositional Learning Analytics Context
295
component of the blend: the use of the two e-
tutorials SOWISO (https://sowiso.nl/) and
MyStatLab (MSL) (Nguyen et al., 2016; Tempelaar
et al., 2013; Tempelaar et al., 2015; Tempelaar,
Rienties, et al., 2017). This design is based on the
philosophy of student-centred education, placing the
responsibility for making educational choices
primarily on the student. Since most of the learning
takes place during self-study outside class through
the e-tutorials or other learning materials, class time
is used to discuss solving advanced problems. Thus,
the instructional format shares most characteristics
of the flipped-classroom design. Using and
achieving good scores in the e-tutorial practice
modes is incentivized by providing bonus points for
good performance in quizzes that are taken every
two weeks and consist of items that are drawn from
the same item pools applied in the practising mode.
This approach was chosen to encourage students
with limited prior knowledge to make intensive use
of the e-tutorials.
The subject of this study is the full 2016/2017
cohort of students (1093 students). A large diversity
of the student population was present: only 19%
were educated in the Dutch high school system,
against 81% being international students, with 50
nationalities present. A large share of students was
of European nationality, with only 3.9% of students
from outside Europe. High school systems in Europe
differ strongly, most particularly in the teaching of
mathematics and statistics. Therefore, it is crucial
that this introductory module is flexible and allows
for individual learning paths. Students spend on
average 24 hours in SOWISO and 32 hours in MSL,
which is 30% to 40% of the available time of 80
hours for learning both topics.
2.2 Instruments and Procedure
Both e-tutorial systems SOWISO and MSL follow a
test-directed learning and practising approach. Each
step in the learning process is initiated by a question,
and students are encouraged to (attempt to) answer
each question. If a student does not master a
question (completely), she/he can either ask for hints
to solve the problem step-by-step, or ask for a fully
worked example. After receiving feedback, a new
version of the problem loads (parameter based) to
allow the student to demonstrate his/her newly
acquired mastery. Students’ revealed preferences for
learning strategies are related to their learning
dispositions, as we demonstrated in previous
research (Nguyen et al., 2016; Tempelaar et al.,
2017, 2017b) for the use of worked-examples in
SOWISO), and the use of worked-examples in MSL
(Tempelaar, 2017). This study extends Nguyen et al.
(2016) and Tempelaar et al. (2017, 2017b) by
investigating three learning strategies in the
SOWISO tool: worked examples, and supported and
tutored problem-solving.
Figure 1 demonstrates the implementation of the
alternative learning strategies students can opt for a
sample exercise:
Check: the untutored problem-solving
approach, offering only correctness
feedback after problem-solving;
Hint: the tutored problem-solving approach,
offering feedback and hints to assist the
student in the several problem-solving
steps;
Solution: the worked-examples approach;
Theory: asking for a short explanation of
the mathematical principle.
Figure 1: Sample of SOWISO exercise with the options
Check, Theory, Solution, and Hint.
Our study combines trace data of the SOWISO e-
tutorial with self-report survey data measuring
learning dispositions. Trace data is both of product
and process type (Azevedo et al., 2013). SOWISO
reporting options of trace data are very broad,
requiring making selections from the data. First, all
dynamic trace data were aggregated over time, to
arrive at static, full course period accounts of trace
data. Second, from the large array of trace variables,
a selection was made by focusing on process
variables most strongly connected to alternative
CSEDU 2018 - 10th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
296
Table 1: Descriptive statistics of four times four sub-samples of students achieving at least 70% mastery level.
Group N Mastery #Attempts #Hints #Examples
HintsQ1&ExamplesQ1 72 96.2% 340 4.3 38.7
HintsQ1&ExamplesQ2 41 97.8% 418 4.8 88.7
HintsQ1&ExamplesQ3 59 98.2% 534 4.2 139.9
HintsQ1&ExamplesQ4 59 97.7% 729 4.5 157.7
HintsQ2&ExamplesQ1 64 97.5% 367 20.8 41.9
HintsQ2&ExamplesQ2 55 99.2% 432 21.8 85.3
HintsQ2&ExamplesQ3 39 99.7% 520 21.0 134.5
HintsQ2&ExamplesQ4 45 98.8% 758 20.3 269.9
HintsQ3&ExamplesQ1 53 97.0% 370 57.4 48.5
HintsQ3&ExamplesQ2 53 98.8% 433 53.8 88.6
HintsQ3&ExamplesQ3 56 99.3% 528 56.7 136.1
HintsQ3&ExamplesQ4 52 99.5% 786 59.5 275.4
HintsQ4&ExamplesQ1 33 99.2% 486 135.8 49.1
HintsQ4&ExamplesQ2 60 98.4% 476 137.9 89.7
HintsQ4&ExamplesQ3 61 99.3% 587 157.2 140.8
HintsQ4&ExamplesQ4 58 99.4% 764 174.9 249.1
Total 860 98.4% 530 58.1 135.5
learning strategies. In total, five trace variables
were selected:
Mastery in the tool, the proportion of
exercises successfully solved as product
indicator;
#Attempts: total number of attempts of
individual exercises;
#Hints: the total number of Hints called for.
#Examples: total number of worked-out
examples called;
To disentangle the effects of learning intensity
from learning strategy, we restricted the sample to
those students who have been very active in the e-
tutorial and achieved at least a 70% mastery level
(that is, successfully solved at least 162 of the 231
exercises): 860 of the 1080 students. Rather than
applying advanced statistical techniques to create
different profiles of using worked examples, as in
Gašević and coauthors (Gašević, Jovanović, et al.
2017; Gašević, Mirriahi, et al., 2017) or our previous
research (Nguyen et al., 2016; Tempelaar, et al.,
2017, 2017a), we applied quartile splits: the selected
students were split into four equal-sized groups
according to intensity of using worked examples, as
well the intensity of using hints. Table 1 provides
descriptive statistics of these four times four sub-
samples.
The operationalization of revealed preferences
for learning strategies follow these quartile splits.
The revealed preference for the worked-examples
strategy is operationalized as students calling for a
lot of examples, ending up in the higher quarters of
the quartile-split for examples. The revealed
preference for the tutored problem-solving strategy
is operationalized as calling for a large number of
hints, thus ending in the higher quarters of the
quartile-split for hints. As is clear from Table 1,
revealed preferences for learning strategies are not
disjunct. A student can combine the strategies of
worked-examples and tutored problem-solving,
calling for an above-average number of hints as well
as above average number of examples.
The strategy of untutored problem-solving is a
necessary component of any of the revealed
preferences, since students can only build mastery
through untutored problem-solving, and the students
included in this analysis all obtained high mastery
levels.
Mastery level is indeed invariant over groups,
and never below 96%: there is a large majority of
students in all sub-samples that reached full mastery.
There is considerable variation in the use of hints,
and the use of examples. The use of hints and
examples seems only weakly associated, except for
the quarter of students using most hints: HintsQ4. In
that quarter, the use of hints and examples is
positively correlated (in HintsQ4, the correlation of
hints and examples equals 0.23, against 0.07 in all
four quarters).
In this study, we will focus on a selection with
regard to the self-report surveys measuring student
learning dispositions. More than a dozen were
administered, ranging from affective learning
emotions to cognitive learning processing strategies:
Epistemological self-theories of intelligence;
Epistemological views on role effort in
learning;
Epistemic learning emotions;
Analysing the Use of Worked Examples and Tutored and Untutored Problem-Solving in a Dispositional Learning Analytics Context
297
Cognitive learning processing strategies;
Metacognitive learning regulation strategies;
Subject-specific (math & stats) learning
attitudes;
Academic motivations;
Achievement goals;
Achievement orientations;
Learning activity emotions;
Motivation & Engagement wheel;
Cultural intelligence;
National cultural values; and
Help-seeking behaviour
Due to lack of space, we refer to previous
research for the description of the self-report
instruments measuring learning dispositions
(Nguyen et al., 2016; Tempelaar, Mittelmeier, et al.,
2017; Tempelaar et al., 2015; Tempelaar, Rienties,
et al., 2017). The description of the research
outcomes will focus on specific aspects of learning
dispositions: learning approaches, anxiety and
uncertainty as aspects of students’ attitudes and
learning emotions.
Course performance data is based on the final
written exam, as well as the three intermediate
quizzes. Quiz scores are averaged, and both exam
and quiz are decomposed into two topic scores,
resulting in MathExam, StatsExam, MathQuiz and
StatsQuiz.
3 RESULTS
3.1 Previous Research
In previous research (Nguyen, 2016; Tempelaar,
2017; Tempelaar et al., 2017a, 2017b), we
investigated the role of worked examples in LA
applications and found that a range of dispositions
predict the use of worked examples as a learning
strategy. Demographic variables, student learning
approaches, learning attitudes and learning emotions
influenced the use of worked examples, with effect
sizes up to 7% for individual dispositions. In our
profiling study (Tempelaar et al., 2017) we found
that the use of worked examples and the total
number of attempts to be the two variables shaping
most of the characteristic differences between
different profiles in the use of the e-tutorial. The use
of hints did not strongly contribute to the creation of
the student use profiles. As a consequence, we
expect dispositions to play a less strong role in the
explanation of the use of hints as a learning strategy
than it has in the explanation of the use of worked
examples. This expectation does indeed come true,
and in the reporting of the empirical outcomes in the
next sections, we will focus on the cases where
dispositions matter in the explanation of both
learning strategies, leaving out the cases where the
impact is primarily on the use of worked examples,
that are described in previous research (Nguyen,
2016; Tempelaar, 2017; Tempelaar et al., 2017,
2017a, 2017b).
3.2 Demographics
Demographic variables have no practical
significance in the explanation of the use of hints:
gender and international status have statistically
non-significant relationships with the intensity of
use of hints. Math prior education has a marginally
significant effect with limited size (p-value=.04, eta
squared=1.2%). Differences in national cultural
values follow this pattern, with the single exception
that students from cultures that assign greater value
to long-term orientation tend to apply the learning
strategy of using hints more often than students from
other cultures (p-value=.004, eta squared=1.2%).
3.3 Learning Approaches
Although the use of learning strategies is the explicit
focus of learning approaches frameworks, the
learning strategy of supported problem solving by
using hints was not adequately captured in our
learning approaches instrument. Hence, we found no
differences in learning approaches for the use of
hints. In the use of worked examples, there were
significant differences for both the deep and
stepwise processing strategies, and self-regulation of
learning.
3.4 Prior Knowledge
Differences induced by different levels of prior math
schooling are enlarged in the first measurement of
cognitive type: the math entry test, administered at
the very start of the course. The score of diagnostic
test was strongly associated with the intensity of
using hints, and the use of worked examples.
Significance levels are .006, <.001 and .018 for the
hints effect, the examples effect, and the interaction
effect, respectively, with a total effect size of eta
squared=11.9%. Figure 2 provides a graphical
illustration of the effects in the several quarters,
where we applied reversed scaling to the several
quarters to facilitate readability. Students with the
highest prior knowledge levels tend to use fewer
CSEDU 2018 - 10th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
298
hints and fewer examples than the other students.
However, there was more consistency in the pattern
for the use of examples, than that for the use of
hints: in the quarter of students with the highest
intensity of using examples, both the Q1 and Q4
quarters of hint use demonstrate low levels of prior
knowledge.
Figure 2: Quarter differences for prior math knowledge, as
measured by a diagnostic test (reversed scaling).
3.5 Learning Attitudes
Since learning attitudes as Affect and Cognitive
Competence are associated with levels of prior
knowledge, it is to be expected that the intensity of
use of both learning strategies is associated with
learning attitudes. That was indeed the case: Affect
(p-value hints<.001, p-value examples<.001, total
eta squared=8.9%), Cognitive competence (p-value
hints<.001, p-value examples<.001, total eta
squared=8.8%) demonstrated clear linear effects in
the absence of interaction effects. Value and Interest
had no role in explaining difference in strategy use,
whereas the NoDifficulty variable was only weakly
associated with both strategies (p-value hints=.034,
p-value examples=.008, total eta squared=2.9%),
and the Effort variable is associated with only the
examples strategy (p-value hints=.461, p-value
examples=.002, total eta squared=3.5%). Figure 3
provides a graphical presentation for the case of
Affect. As in the previous figure, we see that the
highest levels of Affect are to be found in the group
of students who use both hints and examples least
frequently and that intensive use of both strategies is
associated with low levels of Affect.
Figure 3: Quarter differences for learning attitude Affect
(reversed scaling).
3.6 Epistemic Emotions
Epistemic emotions demonstrated group differences
for the negative emotions Confusion (p-value
hints=.004, p-value examples<.001, total eta
squared=7.0%) and Frustration (p-value hints<.001,
p-value examples<.001, total eta squared=6.4%).
Frustration was one of the few disposition variables
that was associated with the use of hints (partial eta-
squared=2.7%) more than with the use of examples
(partial eta-squared=2.4%). Epistemic Enjoyment
makes an even stronger case: here the only
significant relationship is with the use of hints (p-
value hints=.001, p-value examples=.083, total eta
squared=4.8%). Figure 4 demonstrates the
association for Epistemic Frustration, Figure 5 that
for Epistemic Enjoyment.
Figure 4: Quarter differences for Epistemic Frustration
(non-reversed scaling).
Analysing the Use of Worked Examples and Tutored and Untutored Problem-Solving in a Dispositional Learning Analytics Context
299
Figure 5: Quarter differences for Epistemic Enjoyment
(reversed scaling).
3.7 Learning Outcomes
In the main outcome variable of the learning
process, Math Exam or the achievement in the math
section of the final written exam, only associations
with the learning strategy of using examples can be
found, be it that the interaction term is significant (p-
value hints=.106, p-value examples<.001, p-value
interaction=.013, total eta squared=10.9%). Figure 6
provides a graphical description: math exam score is
generally increasing for less intensive use of
examples, but the pattern is not identical for all
quarters of hint use intensity. Specifically, in
students in the third quarter of hint use intensity, the
use of examples and performance seem fairly
unrelated.
Figure 6: Quarter differences for Math Exam score
(reversed scaling).
4 DISCUSSION AND
CONCLUSION
Existing studies into the efficiency of alternative
learning strategies, both in lab settings (Renkl, 2014)
and in classroom settings (McLaren et al., 2014,
2016), point in the direction of worked-examples
being superior to tutored and untutored problem-
solving. These are generic conclusions, which do not
differentiate between types of academic tasks or
types of students. The main contribution of this
study was the emphasis on the individual
differences: learning dispositions make a difference,
academic tasks make a difference. Allowing for
individual differences and task differences also
changes the first order conclusions.
Regarding the first research question, we found
that students who had less prior knowledge sought
more support from both worked-examples and hints.
Similarly: students who experienced more negative
epistemic emotions such as confusion and
frustration, examples of mal-adaptive dispositions,
sought more support from both worked-examples
and hints. Students who scored higher in the prior
knowledge test usually took on the task by
themselves without seeking help from hints or
examples. At the same time, students who used
fewer hints and worked examples scored higher on
the math exam (second research question). This
implies that worked-examples are only superior to
tutored and untutored problem-solving when the
latter two learning strategies are not sufficient to
achieve proficiency. The initial acquisition of
complex knowledge is an example of such a context.
In cases the cognitive challenges of the learning
tasks are less, this superiority may break down, and
worked-examples may be less efficient learning
strategies than problem-solving approaches.
Transferring the findings of the Renkl (2014) and
the McLaren et al. (2014, 2016) studies to our
context, suggests that the superiority of the worked-
examples strategy may be the result of the tasks
offered to the participants of these studies to be of
such type that students in their studies had little or
no prior knowledge. Our context has been different:
given the wide variety of the tasks and the large
diversity in prior knowledge of students, there exists
a wide range of relevant prior knowledge levels for
any task at hand. In such a context, where students
are expected to demonstrate mastery, mastery that
can only be acquired in the untutored problem-
solving mode, the use of examples and hints is
inevitably a roundabout route, adding inefficiency to
the most direct way to mastery. That route of using
CSEDU 2018 - 10th International Conference on Computer Supported Education
300
tutored problem-solving and worked-examples is
taken by students who assessed the direct way of
untutored problem-solving to be -still- impassable,
explaining the relationship with prior knowledge.
The current study has focussed on individual
differences between students in their preference for
learning strategies, and the relationship with learning
dispositions. In future research, we intend to
additionally include the task dimension, by
investigating student preference for learning
strategies as a function of both individual differences
in learning dispositions and task characteristics.
REFERENCES
Azevedo, R., Harley, J., Trevors, G., Duffy, M., Feyzi-
Behnagh, R., Bouchet, F., et al. (2013). Using trace
data to examine the complex roles of cognitive,
metacognitive, and emotional self-regulatory
processes during learning with multi-agents systems.
In R. Azevedo & V. Aleven (Eds.), International
handbook of metacognition and learning technologies,
427–449. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Springer.
Buckingham Shum, S. & Deakin Crick, R. (2012).
Learning Dispositions and Transferable Competencies:
Pedagogy, Modelling and Learning Analytics. In S.
Buckingham Shum, D. Gasevic, & R. Ferguson (Eds.).
Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on
Learning Analytics and Knowledge, 92-101. ACM,
New York, NY, USA. DOI:
10.1145/2330601.2330629.
Gašević, D., Jovanović, J., Pardo, A., & Dawson, S.
(2017). Detecting learning strategies with analytics:
Links with self-reported measures and academic
performance. In Journal of Learning Analytics, 4(1),
113–128 DOI: 10.18608/jla.2017.42.10.
Gašević, D., Mirriahi, N., Dawson, S., & Joksimović, S.
(2017). Effects of instructional conditions and
experience on the adoption of a learning tool
Computers in Human Behavior, 67, 207–220. DOI:
10.1016/j.chb.2016.10.026.
McLaren, B. M., van Gog, T., Ganoe, C., Karabinos, M.,
& Yaron, D. (2016). The efficiency of worked
examples compared to erroneous examples, tutored
problem solving, and problem solving in classroom
experiments. Computers in Human Behavior, 55, 87-
99. DOI: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.08.038.
McLaren, B.M., van Gog, T., Ganoe, C., Yaron, D. &
Karabinos, M. (2014) Exploring the assistance
dilemma: Comparing instructional support in
examples and problems. In S. Trausan-Matu et al.
(Eds.) Proceedings of the Twelfth International
Conference on Intelligent Tutoring Systems (ITS-
2014). LNCS 8474. (pp. 354-361). Springer
International Publishing Switzerland.
Nguyen, Q., Tempelaar, D.T., Rienties, B., & Giesbers, B.
(2016). What learning analytics based prediction
models tell us about feedback preferences of students.
In Amirault, R., & Visser, Y., (Eds.). (2016). e-
Learners and Their Data, Part 1: Conceptual,
Research, and Exploratory Perspectives. Quarterly
Review of Distance Education. 17(3).
Renkl, A. (2014). The Worked Examples Principle in
Multimedia Learning. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The
Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning, 391-
412. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Rienties, B., Cross, S., & Zdrahal, Z (2017). Implementing
a Learning Analytics Intervention and Evaluation
Framework: What Works? In B. Kei Daniel (Ed.), Big
data and learning analytics: Current theory and
practice in higher education, 147–166. Cham:
Springer International Publishing. DOI: 10.1007/978-
3-319-06520-5_10.
Tempelaar, D. T., Cuypers, H., Van de Vrie, E., Heck, A.,
Van der Kooij, H. (2013). Formative Assessment and
Learning Analytics. In D. Suthers & K. Verbert (Eds.),
Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on
Learning Analytics and Knowledge, 205-209. New
York: ACM. DOI: 10.1145/2460296.2460337.
Tempelaar, D. (2017). How dispositional Learning
Analytics helps understanding the worked-example
principle. In D. G. Sampson, J. M. Spector, D.
Ifenthaler, & P. Isaias (Eds.), Proceedings 14th
International Conference on Cognition and
Exploratory Learning in Digital Age (CELDA 2017),
pp. 117-124. International Association for
Development of the Information Society, IADIS Press.
Tempelaar, D.T., Mittelmeier, J., Rienties, B., & Nguyen,
Q. (2017). Student profiling in a dispositional learning
analytics application using formative assessment,
Computers in Human Behavior (in press). DOI:
10.1016/j.chb.2017.08.010.
Tempelaar, D. T., Rienties, B., & Giesbers, B. (2015). In
search for the most informative data for feedback
generation: Learning Analytics in a data-rich context.
Computers in Human Behavior, 47, 157-167. DOI:
10.1016/j.chb.2014.05.038.
Tempelaar, D.T., Rienties, B., & Nguyen, Q. (2017a).
Towards actionable learning analytics using
dispositions. IEEE Transactions on Education, 10(1),
6-16. DOI:10.1109/TLT.2017.2662679.
Tempelaar, D.T., Rienties, B., & Nguyen, Q. (2017b).
Adding dispositions to create pedagogy-based
Learning Analytics. Zeitschrift für
Hochschulentwicklung, ZFHE, 12(1), 15-35.
Analysing the Use of Worked Examples and Tutored and Untutored Problem-Solving in a Dispositional Learning Analytics Context
301