Predicting Well-being from Different Dimensions of Religiousness:
Initial Application of 4-BDRS Scale in Indonesia
Yonathan Aditya
1
, Riryn Sani
1
, Ihan Martoyo
2
,
3
and Rudy Pramono
2
1
Department of Psychology, Universitas Pelita Harapan, Tangerang, Indonesia
2
Center for Research and Community Development, Universitas Pelita Harapan, Tangerang, Indonesia
3
Reformed Theological Seminary Indonesia (STTRI), Kemang, Jakarta, Indonesia
Keywords: Well-being, Religiousness, 4-BDRS, Islam, Christianity.
Abstract: As a nation based on the belief in one supreme God, Indonesia has depicted indigenous religious
phenomena worthy of scientific analysis. Due to the endorsement of religiousness in Indonesia, prior
research aimed to find empirical evidence on the effects of religiousness on well-being. However, most of
the prior research only focused on one religion, either Islam or Christianity. This paper shows and compares
how religiousness in both Islam and Christianity can predict well-being. This study will also show the
dynamics of different dimensions of religiousness using the Four Basic Dimensions of Religiousness Scale
(4-BDRS), which has been extensively used in the West, in explaining well-being. The empirical evidences
were derived from 614 college students from religious-based universities using 4-BDRS and the Satisfaction
with Life Scale (SWLS). The result demonstrated that for both Muslims and Christians, total religiousness
has a significant positive correlation with well-being (r=.34, ρ<.05; r=.39, ρ<.05). However, the results of
multiple regression test revealed that for Muslims, only Belonging has a significant effect on well-being
(β=.16, ρ<.05) while for Christians only Believing (β=.17, ρ<.05) and Belonging (β=.36, ρ<.05) have a
significant effect on well-being. Independent sample t-test indicated that Christian college students show a
significantly higher Belonging dimension (M=5.16, SD=1.23, N=275), than Muslim students (M=4.89,
SD=1.14, N=339), t (612) = -2.91, p<0.005. On the other hand, Muslim college students (M=6.15, SD=.77,
N=339) display a significantly higher Behaving dimension than Christians (M=5.91, SD=.91, N=275),
t (535.2) = 3.41, p <.05. These results shed some lights on how Muslims and Christians can be similar and
different at the same time, regarding their religiousness and satisfaction with life. Efforts to increase the
well-being of religious students might give more attention to the Believing and Belonging dimension of
religiousness.
1 INTRODUCTION
In recent times, numerous studies have been
conducted to investigate the effects of religiousness
on well-being. However, most of these studies were
done on Christian populations in the West. Only
recently have studies been done on Muslim
populations (Abu-Raiya 2013). Saroglou (2013), on
the other hand, wrote that the relationship between
religiousness and well-being was affected by the
type of religion and culture. This research studies
the interplay between religiousness and well-being
in the two largest religions in Indonesia, namely
Islam and Christianity, using a comprehensive
measure of religiousness created by Saroglou,
namely The Four Basic Dimensions of
Religiousness Scale (4-BDRS) (Saroglou 2011).
Religiousness is a construct to measure an
individual’s level of interest on religion (Saroglou
2013). Considering that religion is a
multidimensional construct, a good measure of
religiousness should be multidimensional as well
(Koenig 2018). A single item index of religiousness
or scale that focuses only on one dimension of
religiousness fails to understand the complexity of
religion. To answer this problem, Saroglou (2011)
analyzed all measures of religiousness and
concluded that all religions have four dimensions of
religiousness: Believing, Bonding, Behaving, and
Belonging. Saroglou then created the 4-BDRS,
which consisted of these four dimensions. Believing
458
Aditya, Y., Sani, R., Martoyo, I. and Pramono, R.
Predicting Well-being from Different Dimensions of Religiousness: Initial Application of 4-BDRS Scale in Indonesia.
DOI: 10.5220/0008590804580463
In Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Psychology in Health, Educational, Social, and Organizational Settings (ICP-HESOS 2018) - Improving Mental Health and Harmony in
Global Community, pages 458-463
ISBN: 978-989-758-435-0
Copyright
c
2020 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
is a set of beliefs about external transcendence (one
or many). Bonding is related to the emotional parts
of religiousness that unites an individual to the
external transcendence. Behaving is concerned with
norms and moral requirements, while Belonging
refers to cohesion with a community.
These four dimensions of religiousness reflect
four psychological processes, namely cognitive,
moral, emotional, and social, and they are not
limited to any single religious tradition. Therefore,
the four dimensions are presumably present in all
religions and culture (Saroglou, Vassilis and Cohen,
2013). The universality of the 4-BDRS makes this
measurement useful in cross-cultural and cross-
religion research. Although these four dimensions
are universal, the magnitude and correlations among
these four dimensions may vary from one religion to
another, as well as from one culture to another. The
same is also true on the effect of religiousness on
well-being (Saroglou 2011).
A majority of research found that religiousness
has a significant positive effect on well-being
(Koenig 2018). Religion affects well-being through
many ways, both psychologically and socially.
People high in religiousness tend to have better
coping strategies, which in turn reduces their levels
of stress (Ju, et al., 2018; Gebauer, et al., 2012).
Religion gives meaning in life, thus people with a
high level of religiousness perceive a better purpose
in life (Wang, et al., 2016). Furthermore, religious
communities tend to support their members.
Therefore, people high in religiousness tend to have
better social support. Low stress levels, purpose in
life, and greater social support are linked to better
well-being.
Research on the effects of religiousness on well-
being among Muslims showed similar results.
Muslims with high religiousness tend to have better
indicators of well-being such as more adaptive
coping, better relationships with others, and higher
satisfaction in life (Aflakseir and Coleman, 2009;
Ghorbani and Watson, 2006). However, considering
that many studies were not done using
multidimensional measure, it is difficult to know
which dimensions of religiousness play more
significant role on well-being (Abu-Raiya, 2013;
Koenig, 2018). A research done by Dimitrova and
Espinosa (2016) on Mexicans found that all
dimensions of 4-BDRS have a significant effect on
well-being, while the study of Dimitrova (2014)
among Dutch and Italians found no significant
correlations among all dimensions of 4-BDRS with
well-being. These results support the idea that
culture affects the relations between religiousness
and well-being. However, these studies were done
on Mexicans and Europeans and the majority of the
participants were not Muslim. Therefore, many
scholars in psychology of religion suggested
research on the effect of religiousness on well-being
in other religions and cultures other than Western
Christian populations (Abu-Raiya, 2013; Koenig,
2018; Saroglou, Vassilis and Cohen, 2013)
This study focuses on the relationship between
religiousness and well-being within the context of
Muslim and Christian college students in Indonesia.
Indonesia is a multi-ethnic and multi-religion
country. There are 1340 ethnics groups and 6
religions that are endorsed by the government.
According to the last census, the total population of
Indonesia is more than 237 millions with 87% of
Muslims and 7% Christians respectively. This makes
Indonesia the largest Muslim country in the world
(Badan Pusat Statistik Republik Indonesia, 2010).
However, it is important to note that Indonesia is
based on Pancasila (The five principles) as the state
foundation and not on any religion. The freedom of
religion is guaranteed by the constitution. Despite
some incidents of religious intolerance, in general
people from different religions are able to live
peacefully in Indonesia. This is reflected in the result
of the latest religious harmony survey done by
Religious Affairs Ministry which found index of
religious harmony in Indonesia is good (Didah,
2018). Therefore, with its unique culture, it will be
interesting to observe how Muslim and Christian
religiousness can affect the well-being of religious
individuals in Indonesia.
In general, Evangelical Christianity in Indonesia
seems to focus on doctrinal differences (Hoon,
2013) thus the cognitive (Believing) dimension is
emphasized. However, Charismatic Christian
communities are known for their emphasis on the
affective (Bonding) dimension through their livelier
style of worship. Non-Charismatic churches often
criticize the Charismatics to be too emotional
(Hocken, 2010). However, these two groups are not
mutually exclusive, as some churches attempt to
care for both the cognitive and affective in their
services.
Islam, on the other hand, focuses on religious
practices (the five pillars of Islam): (1) Belief in one
God, (2) Pray regularly (salat), (3) Give generously
to the poor, (4) Fast in the month of Ramadan, and if
possible (5) Pilgrimage to Mecca (Saeed, 2008,
p. 73).
However, there is a minority strand of Islam,
called Sufism, which concerns more on the spiritual
Predicting Well-being from Different Dimensions of Religiousness: Initial Application of 4-BDRS Scale in Indonesia
459
hidden meaning of Qur’anic verses than the physical
practices. Saeed describes Sufism as follows:
Proponents of mystical exegesis emphasized the
spiritual aspects of Islam, rather than the political,
legal and worldly dimensions. Sufi scholars often
preferred to explore questions regarding
knowledge of God or the nature of human
existence and its relation to the divine. They
believed that the mystical allusions in the Qur’anic
text were related most closely to the human
spiritual condition and were impossible to
understand through superficial readings or
arguments over points of law and theology. Thus,
in mystical exegesis, the spiritual and inner
meanings of the Qur’an were considered
paramount (Saeed, 2008, p. 206).
In the Sufi groups (tarekat), the chanting of prayers
and the counting of beads are not the focus, but they
are practiced for closeness to God and understanding
of His divinity (Pringle, 2010, p. 129). Although the
Indonesian traditionalists might see a risk of heresy
in mystical practices, Islam is clearly capable of
expressing the complete four dimensions of
religiousness, including the affective dimension
(Bonding).
Based on previous studies on the effects of
religousness on well-being, this current study
hypothesizes that both Muslim and Christian
religousness will have a significant positive effect on
well-being. However, the relationships of the 4-
BDRS dimensions to well-being in both religions
could not be predicted as there is no known prior
research on this subject.
2 METHOD
2.1 Participants
The total respondents who completed usable
questionnaires were 614 college students composed
of 339 Muslims and 275 Christians. These
respondents were taken from two Muslim based
universities, one Christian university, and two non-
religious based universities in Jabodetabek area
(Jakarta, Bogor, Depok, Tangerang and Bekasi).
Most of the students (90%) were between the ages of
18-21 years old. 4% were 17 years old, 5% were
between 22-23, and the other 1 % were between 24-
28 years old. 25% of the participants were male
while 75% were female. Regarding ethnicity, 36%
of the participants reported being of mixed ethnicity,
20% as Javanese, 19% as Chinese, 9% as
Sundanese, while the other 17% were from Betawi,
Batak, Manado, Ambon, Timor and Papua.
2.2 Measures
Religiousness. The Four Basic Dimensions of
Religiousness Scale (4-BDRS) was used to measure
religiousness. This scale was developed by Saroglou
(2011) and measures 4 dimensions of religiousness.
Believing (3 items, e.g.,
I feel attached to religion
because it helps me to have a purpose in my life”).
Bonding (3 items, e.g., “I like religious
ceremonies”). Behaving (3 items, e.g., “I am
attached to the religion for the values and ethics it
endorses”). Belonging (3 items, e.g., “In religion, I
enjoy belonging to a group/community”). The sum
of 12 items results in the total score of religiousness.
In this study, internal reliability measured by
Cronbach’s alpha was .89 for Muslims and .91 for
Christians sample.
Well-being. Well-being was measured using The
Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) (Diener, et al.,
1985). This scale consisted of five items (e.g., “I am
satisfied with my life”) on a 7-point Likert scale. In
this study, internal reliability measured by
Cronbach’s alpha was .71 for Muslims and .78 for
the Christians sample.
3 RESULT
Table 1: Means (and standard deviations) of samples
based on religion.
Total
Religiousne
ss
Muslims
(n=339)
Christians
(n=275)
Group
Compariso
n
5.63
(.77)
5.63 (.86) t (553.51)
= .02, ρ
>.05
Believing 6.07
(.87)
5.95 (.91) t (612)
=1.56,
ρ >.05
Bonding 5.43
(1.02)
5.49
(1.03)
t (612) = -
.79,
ρ
>.05
Behaving 6.15
(.77)
5.91 (.91) t (535.2) =
3.41, ρ
<.01
Belonging 4.89
(1.14)
5.16
(1.23)
t (612) = -
2.91,
ρ <.01
Well-being 4.56
(.94)
4.54
(1.06)
t (550.8) =
.27,
ρ >.05
ICP-HESOS 2018 - International Conference on Psychology in Health, Educational, Social, and Organizational Settings
460
Independent sample t-test (Table 1) shows that
Muslim students (M=6.15, SD=.77, N=339) have a
significantly higher Behaving dimension than
Christian Students (M=5.91, SD=.91, N=275),
t (535.2) = 3.41, ρ <.05. On the other hand, Christian
students (M=5.16, SD=1.23, N=275) have a higher
score in Belonging than Muslim students (M=4.89,
SD=1.14, N=339), t (612) = -2.91, ρ<0.005. The
correlations in Table 2 show that for both Muslims
and Christians, total religiousness and all of its four
dimensions indicate significant positive correlations
with well-being. However, multiple regressions test
in Table 3 revealed that not all dimensions of
religiousness show a significant effect on well-
being. For Muslim students, only Belonging (β=.16,
ρ<.05) was found to have a significant positive
effect on well-being. In contrast, only Believing
(β=.17, ρ<.05) and Belonging (β=.36, ρ<.05) have a
significant positive effect on well-being among
Christian students.
Table 2: Correlations between religiousness, its
dimensions, and well-being based on religions
SWLS
Muslims Christians
r
Ρ
r
ρ
Total 4-
BDRS
.34 <.05 .39 <.05
Believing .29 <.05 .31 <.05
Bonding .22 <.05 .31 <.05
Behavin
g
.30 <.05 .25 <.05
Belon
g
in
g
.28 <.05 .41 <.05
Table 3: Multiple regressions analysis of religiousness and
well-being based on religions
SWLS
Muslims Christians
β ρ β ρ
Believing .13 >.05 .17 <.05
Bonding -.01 >.05 .03 >.05
Behaving .14 >.05 -.10 >.05
Belonging .16 <.05 .36 <.05
4 DISCUSSION
The result of this study reveals that religiousness has
a positive effect on well-being for both Muslim and
Christian students. This means that both Muslim and
Christian students with higher level of religiousness
will have better well-being than that with lower level
of religiousness. This is similar with the result from
previous research (Dimitrova, R., Espinosa 2017).
However, the correlations between the dimensions
of religiousness and the total religiousness to well-
being in this current study are higher than that of
Dimitrova and Espinosa (2017) who did their study
on Mexican samples. In this current research, the
correlation scores are above .2, while in the previous
study the correlation scores are below .2. This result,
however, is not the same with the result of the study
done on Dutch and Italians which found no
significant correlations between religiousness and
well-being (Dimitrova 2014).
These facts highlight the effect of culture on the
relationship between religiousness and well-being. It
is likely that the wealth of a nation moderates the
effect of religiousness on well-being as revealed in
the survey result of Pew Research Center
(Theodorou, 2015). This survey found that people in
the poorer countries were more religious than people
in more wealthy countries. Among these four
countries (Italy, the Netherlands, Mexico,
Indonesia), Italy and the Netherlands are far more
prosperous than the other two, and they do not
consider religion to be very important in life.
Conversely, Indonesia is the least developed country
compared to the other three countries. Almost all
Indonesian believe in spiritual life (God, life after
death, soul, heaven and hell) that makes Indonesia
the world’s most believing country (Gallup
International, 2017). This fact explains the more
significant effect of religiousness to well-being in
Indonesia compared to the prior mentioned wealthier
countries.
Although the importance for religiousness is
decreasing with increasing GDP in a country, it is
interesting to note that there are some outliers to this
trend. The US is still showing a moderately high
religiousness compared to many European countries
despite the wealth of the nation (Theodorou, 2015).
As a nation based upon the belief in one supreme
God, Indonesia is one of the most believing
countries in the world (Gallup International, 2017).
God is in the every day conversation of the people.
Indonesian culture is collectivist as well. Geert
Hofstede’s studies into the culture of different
countries showed that Indonesia (individualism
index = 14), is more collectivist than Mexico
(individualism index = 30), and far more collectivist
than European countries, such as Italy
(individualism index =76) or the Netherlands
(individualism index = 80) (Hofstede, 2010, p. 95-
97). Hence, people tend to look for support from
their relatives, peer, or community (Markus, H.R., &
Predicting Well-being from Different Dimensions of Religiousness: Initial Application of 4-BDRS Scale in Indonesia
461
Kitayama, 1991; Kuo, 2013). Since the belief in God
is infused in every part of Indonesian culture, people
will be conditioned to use religious coping in time of
stress (Pargament, K.I., Falb, M.D., Ano, G.G. &
Wachholtz 2013). This is also one of the reasons as
to why Belonging shared the highest variance of
well-being for both Muslim and Christian students.
Jakarta and its surroundings display a kind of
individualistic, secularized environment. So, it
seems that religious Belonging can provide the sense
of rootedness for college students, who live in
Jakarta greater area that is becoming more like other
Western cities in terms of individualism and
competitiveness. Through religious belonging, they
can satisfy their basic need for social ties in a shared
value system. This result replicated the research of
ten Kate et. al. (2017) who found that Belonging
dimension of religiousness has a significant effect on
well-being for Catholics in the secularized and
pluralistic society of the Netherlands.
However, Muslim and Christian students display
differences in the dimensions of religiousness that
influence well-being. For Muslim students, only
Belonging indicates significant positive effect on the
well-being of Muslim students. On the other hand,
for Christian students, only Believing and Belonging
indicate significant positive effect. Bonding does not
show a significant effect on well-being for both
Muslim and Christian students. This result was
replicated in some previous research that found the
importance of Believing and Belonging dimensions
for life satisfaction (Kate, J.T., Koster, W.D.
2017)(Saputra, Goei & Lanawati 2017). Both studies
have similar non-Charismatic respondents, who tend
to focus on the cognitive aspects of religiousness.
In this study, Bonding did not show a significant
effect on well-being both for Muslim and Christian
students. Perhaps future research should be
conducted on Christian Charismatic communities or
Islamic Sufism, in which expressions of emotion and
mysticism are more deeply felt and explored.
The differences between the religious
dimensions that affect well-being between Muslim
and Christian students also call for further elaborate
research. Why is it that the Behaving dimension,
which is shown more by the Muslim students
(Table 1), does not predict well-being? Could it be
that because the Indonesian (and Asian) culture is
more collectivist than Western cultures, that the
Belonging dimension is more influential in
determining well-being.
5 CONCLUSIONS
The results of this study support the hypotheses for
both Muslim and Christian college students: The
higher their total religiousness, the higher their well-
being. However, further analysis revealed that only
the dimension of Belonging has a significant
positive effect for both Muslim and Christian
students. The dimension of Believing has a
significant positive effect only for Christians, while
the other two dimensions of The Four Basic
Dimensions of Religiousness (Bonding and
Behaving) did not have a significant effect.
This result emphasizes the effect of culture and
religion on the relationship between religiousness
and well-being. Both culture and religion can play a
significant role. Religious belonging can increase the
well-being of religious individuals by providing
social ties and a sense of identity within a
community with shared values. This happens for
both Muslim and Christian students. The Believing
dimension, on the other hand, can provide a coherent
meaning for one’s life, which in turn increases well-
being. This happens with the Christian students in
our samples. The strong collectivist character of
Indonesian culture may accentuate the result of this
study compared to the more individualistic Western
culture.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This research is supported by the Indonesian
Ministry of Research and Higher Education No:
021/KM/PNT/2018, March 6, 2018; Kontrak
Penelitian Dasar Unggulan Perguruan Tinggi No:
147/LPPM-UPH/IV/2018.
REFERENCES
Abu-Raiya, H. 2013. The psychology of Islam: Current
empirically based knowledge, potential challenges,
and directions for future research. In: K. Pargament,
ed. APA handbook of psychology, religion, and
spirituality: Context, theory, and research, vol. 1. pp.
681–95.
Aflakseir, A., & Coleman, P.G. 2009. The influence of
religious coping on mental health of disabled Iranian
war veterans. Mental health, Religion, and Culture,
12 (2), pp. 175–90.
Badan Pusat Statistik Republik Indonesia 2007, Berita
Resmi Statistik, 2014 (28), pp. 1–5.
Didah, 2014, Indeks Kerukunan Umat Beragama di
ICP-HESOS 2018 - International Conference on Psychology in Health, Educational, Social, and Organizational Settings
462
Indonesia Tahun 2017 Kategori Baik. Available at: <
https://kemenag.go.id/berita/read/507241/indeks-
kerukunan-umat-beragama-di-indonesia-tahun-2017-
kategori-baik> [accessed 11
th
October 2018].
Dimitrova, R., Espinosa, A.C.D. 2017. Factorial structure
and measurement invariance of the Four Basic
Dimensions of Religiousness Scale among Mexican
Males and Females. Psychology of Religion and
Spirituality, 9 (2), pp. 231–8.
Dimitrova, R. 2014, The Four Basic Dimensions of
Religiousness and life satisfaction among Dutch and
Italian young adults. In: M.Mickutcha & F. Saraccino,
eds. Life Satisfaction: Perceptions, Social Influences
and Implications for Long-Term Health. New York:
Nova Publishers, pp. 17–28.
Gallup International 2016, End of Year 2016: Global
report on religion. [pdf] Available at: <
http://gallup.com.pk/wp-
content/uploads/2017/04/Global-Report-on-Religion-
2.pdf> [Accessed 6 June 2018].
Gebauer, J.E., Sedikides, C., & Neberich, W. 2012.
Religiosity, social self-esteem, and psychological
adjustment on the cross-cultural specificity of the
psychological benefits of religiosity. Psychological
Science, 23 (2), pp. 158–60.
Ghorbani, N., Watson, P.J. 2006. Religious orientation
types in Iranian Muslims: Differences in alexithymia,
emotional intelligence, self-consciousness and
psychological adjustment. Review of Religious
Research, 47 (3), pp. 303–10.
Hofstede, G., Hofstede, G.J. & Minkov, M., 2010.
Cultures and Organizations: Software of the Mind.
New York: McGraw-Hill.
Ju, C., Zhang, B., You, X., Alterman, V., & Li, Y. 2018.
Religiousness, social support and subjective well-
being: An exploratory study among adolescents in an
Asian atheist country. International journal of
psychology, 53 (2), pp. 97–106.
Kate, J.T., Koster, W.D., W.J. 2017. The effect of
religiosity on life satisfaction in a secularized context:
Assessing the relevance of believing and belonging.
Review of Religious Research, 59 (2), pp. 135–55.
Koenig, H.G. 2018. Religion and mental health: Research
and clinical application. San Diego: Elsevier.
Kuo, B.C.H. 2013. Collectivism and coping: Current
theories, evidence, and measurements of collective
coping. International Journal of Psyhchology, 48 (3),
pp. 374–8.
Markus, H.R., & Kitayama, S. 1991. Culture and the self:
Implication for cognition, emotion, and motivation’,
Psychological Review. 98 (2), pp. 224–53.
Pargament, K.I., Falb, M.D., Ano, G.G. & Wachholtz,
A.B. 2013. The religous dimension of coping:
Advances in theory, research, and practice. In: R.F.
Paloutzian & C.L. Park, eds. Handbook of the
psychology of religion and spirituality, 2nd ed. New
York: Guilford Press. pp. 560–79.
Saeed, A., 2008. The Qur'an, An Introduction. New York:
Routledge.
Saputra, A., Goei, Y.A. & Lanawati, S. 2017. Hubungan
Believing dan Belonging sebagai dimensi religiusitas
dengan lima dimensi well-being pada mahasiswa di
Tangerang. Jurnal Psikologi Ulayat, 3 (1), pp. 7–17.
Saroglou, Vassilis & Cohen, A.B. 2013. Cultural and
cross-cultural psychology of religion. In: R. F.
Paloutzian & C. L. Park. Handbook of the psychology
of religion and spirituality, 2nd ed. New York:
Guilford Press, pp. 330–53.
Saroglou, V. 201. Believing, bonding, behaving, and
belonging: The big four religious dimensions and
cultural variation. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, 42 (8), pp. 1320–40.
Saroglou, V. 2013. Religion Personality and Social
Behavior. New York: Psychology Press.
Wang, Z., Koenig, H.G., Ma, H., & Al Shohaib, S. 2016,
Religion, purpose in life, social support, and
psychological distress in Chinese University students.
Journal of Religion and Health, 55 (3), pp. 1055–64.
Predicting Well-being from Different Dimensions of Religiousness: Initial Application of 4-BDRS Scale in Indonesia
463