Relationship between Initiation of Gaze Stabilisation and Angle of
Head and Trunk Movement during a Jump with Full Turn
Yusuke Sato
1
, Shuko Torii
2
and Masaharu Sasaki
3
1
College of Commerce, Nihon University, Tokyo, Japan
2
College of Arts and Sciences, University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
3
Faculty of Liberal Arts, Hirosaki Gakuin University, Aomori, Japan
Keywords: Eye Movement, Gaze Behaviour, Gymnastics, Landing, Spatial Orientation.
Abstract: Over time it has become clear that there is a relationship between visual spotting and movement in the air in
gymnasts, but that relationship during basic skills that are simple for skilled gymnasts, such as a jump with
full turn, is still unclear. The aim of this study was to reveal the relationship between the initiation of gaze
stabilisation and the magnitude of body rotation angle during landing. The participants were 10 skilled male
gymnasts. Their eye movements during jumps were measured using electrooculography and their body
movements were recorded using two high-speed digital cameras. The initiation of gaze stabilisation
immediately before landing was determined by combining eye and head movement data. We found various
relationships between initiation of gaze stabilisation and jump movement in gymnasts, such as a positive
correlation between the gaze stabilisation and the head-on-trunk angle at the initiation of gaze stabilisation
and angles of trunk rotation at the landing. The results suggest that gymnasts who can look at locations quicker
before landing might have an advantage in completing rotation, as well as gaining enough time to use visual
information. For achieving early gaze stabilisation, it may be necessary to rotate the head ahead of the trunk.
1 INTRODUCTION
Gymnasts perform in a fixed environment in which
they use visual, vestibular, and somatosensory
information to execute their routine (Sands, 1991). In
particular, visual information is important for
gymnasts to control their landings with precision (Lee
et al., 1992), even when performing basic elements,
such as a jump with full turn (Figure 1) (Rezette and
Amblard, 1985).
The jump with full turn requires a rotation of 360
deg about the longitudinal body axis during a straight
jump. Gymnasts stabilise their gaze immediately
before landing (Sato et al., 2015). Gaze stabilization,
so-called “spotting” (Davlin et al., 2004; Laws et al.,
2002) during landing is especially important for
control of landing (Berthoz and Pozzo, 1994) and the
magnitude of rotation (Heinen et al., 2012a).
Interestingly, there is a possibility that spotting
not only enables movement control using visual
information obtained through gaze stabilisation, but
also directly affects a performer’s movement (Heinen
et al., 2012; Heinen and Vollmer, 2014). For example,
manipulating the fixation point during the takeoff
phase in a back tuck somersault influences the
duration of the flight phase (Heinen and Vollmer,
2014). Heinen et al., (2012b) also reported that there
is a relationship between visual spotting and
movement when performing high bar dismounts; for
example, fixations during the preparatory giant
swings are correlated with flight distance during
dismount. There is an interplay of eye-head
movements with gaze displacement and body
movements even while executing whole-body
rotation without a jump
Figure 1: Jump with full turn (360°).
(Hollands et al., 2004). Therefore, it was
hypothesised that gaze stabilisation immediately
before landing during a jump with full turn correlates
the body movements. The aim of this study was to
reveal the relationship between the initiation of gaze
Sato, Y., Torii, S. and Sasaki, M..
Relationship between Initiation of Gaze Stabilisation and Angle of Head and Trunk Movement during a Jump with Full Turn.
In Proceedings of the 3rd International Congress on Sport Sciences Research and Technology Support (icSPORTS 2015), pages 165-168
ISBN: 978-989-758-159-5
Copyright
c
2015 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
165
stabilisation and the magnitude of body rotation angle
during landing.
Clarifying this problem may contribute to
developing a more effective training programme.
When gymnasts perform aerial movements with
longitudinal axes, a lack of rotation at landing leads
to deductions of score in competitive gymnastics
(Fédération Internationale de Gymnastique, 2013). If
gymnasts can quickly stabilise their gazes, it has a
positive effect on controlling the magnitude of
rotation, an advantage of early gaze stabilisation that
may be suggested in addition to those of previous
studies.
2 METHODS
2.1 Participants
The participants were 10 skilled male gymnasts. After
receiving a written explanation of the aim and content
of the experiment, all participants provided written
informed consent.
2.2 Apparatus and Procedure
The participants performed a jump with full turn in a
gymnasium. A soft mat was put on a floor to ensure
safety. Participants wore comfortable clothes such as
sportswear and were barefoot for all tasks. Before the
experimental trial, markers were attached to the
auricles and acromia of each participant in order to
easily determine head and trunk movements in space.
The experimental task was a jump with full turn.
Participants jumped vertically and rotated 360 deg
about the longitudinal body axis. They stood upright
in the centre of the safety mat and then jumped and
were free to select the direction of rotation. We
instructed the participants to look forward at the
beginning of each trial, and to make a perfect landing.
They were allowed to practice freely before the
experiment and then were prepared for the
experiment. Each participant performed a jump with
full turn three times. The experimenter monitored eye
movement data during the jump, and if the data were
noisy, the gymnasts performed the jump again.
Landing performance was estimated in conformity to
the international competition rules for men’s artistic
gymnastics (Fédération Internationale de
Gymnastique, 2013).
In order to estimate gaze, horizontal eye
movement and head movement were measured. Eye
movement during the jump was clarified by
electrooculography (EOG) using a wireless system
(500 Hz; BioLog DL-4000; S&ME, Tokyo, Japan).
An electrode was attached to the outer canthus of each
eye. Simultaneously, to determine horizontal head
and trunk movement, the experimental trials were
recorded using two high-speed digital cameras
(240f/s, EX-F, Casio, Tokyo, Japan). The cameras
were placed at positions diagonally in front of the
mat.
2.3 Data Acquisition and Analysis
The angle of eye movement was calculated by
calibrating EOG. The recorded movement data were
downloaded to a computer, and measurement points
were digitised using motion analysis software
(Frame-DIASIV, DKH Inc., Tokyo, Japan). The
three-dimensional coordinates of the head and trunk
points were obtained by direct linear transformation
(DLT). The angle of horizontal head movement was
obtained from the line connecting the auricles. The
angle of horizontal trunk movement was obtained
using the line connecting the acromia. The head and
trunk data were filtered using a Butterworth low-pass
filter with a 6-Hz cutoff.
The optical signal was recorded by the cameras,
adding in the PC to synchronise the kinematics and
eye movement data. To synchronise, we use the body
movement data as a basis and picked eye movement
data to synchronise with head data.
Gaze during the jumps was determined by
combining eye and head movement data
(Anastasopoulos et al., 2009). Head-on-trunk
movement was obtained from the angle between the
head and trunk segments. The initiation time of gaze
stabilisation before landing was defined as the point
in time of the completion of gaze shifting in the
rotational direction, and was calculated on the basis
of landing. Landing was defined as the time when the
participant’s foot visibly made contact with the
ground.
Variables pertaining to the performance of each
participant were used for statistical analysis. This
analysis was performed using SPSS 18.0 for
Windows (IBM, Tokyo, Japan). The Pearson
correlation coefficients between head/trunk/head-on-
trunk angles at the time of initiation of gaze
stabilisation and head/trunk angles at the landing and
initiation time of gaze stabilisation were used to
evaluate the relationship between gaze behaviour and
movement kinematics in gymnasts. The level of
significance was set at p < .05.
icSPORTS 2015 - International Congress on Sport Sciences Research and Technology Support
166
3 RESULTS
All gymnasts performed perfect landings. Their eyes
rotated in the direction opposite to that of their head
movement before landing. Almost at the same time,
their gazes started to stabilise. The angles of head and
trunk movement were almost 0 deg at the beginning
of the trial, and after the rotation, the angles had
reached approximately 360 deg.
The mean angle of head movement at the
initiation of gaze stabilisation was 299.71 ± 8.47 deg;
the mean angle of trunk movement at the initiation of
gaze stabilisation was 263.64 ± 22.57 deg; the mean
angle of head-on-trunk at the initiation of gaze
stabilisation was 35.40 ± 19.19 deg; the mean angle
of head movement at the landing was 316.15 ± 17.12
deg, and the mean angle of trunk movement at the
landing was 306.07 ± 18.05 deg. Therefore, head
movement preceded trunk movement at the initiation
of gaze stabilisation.
Figure 2: Example of gaze and head/trunk/head-on-trunk
movements during a jump with full turn.
The analysis revealed certain relationships between
the initiation of gaze stabilisation and the jump
movement in gymnasts. Table 1 shows the correlation
coefficients. According to these results, the initiation
time of gaze stabilisation is negatively correlated with
the head/trunk angle at the initiation of gaze
stabilisation and positively correlated with the head-
on-trunk angle at the initiation of gaze stabilisation,
head angle and trunk angle at landing. Therefore,
when gymnasts execute quick gaze stabilisation
before landing, the angle of head and trunk movement
is smaller at that time and larger at landing.
4 DISCUSSION
The aim of this study was to reveal the relationship
between initiation of gaze stabilisation and movement
kinematics before landing. Although gymnasts use
visual information to land completely during a jump
with turn (Rezette and Amblard, 1985) through gaze
stabilisation (Sato et al., 2014), the relationship
between gaze stabilisation before landing and
magnitude of rotation was unclear. Clarifying this
problem may contribute to developing a more
effective training program.
The gymnasts’ initiation of gaze stabilisation
correlated with all the kinematic variables in this
study. A functional relationship between gaze
behaviour and movement has been reported in recent
years (Heinen et al., 2012b; von Lassberg et al.,
2014). This study’s results also showed that gaze
behaviour is linked to magnitude of body rotation
angle. The gaze stabilisation that occurs before
landing is thought to lead to optimal landing and
precise rotation (Heinen et al., 2012a; Hondzinski and
Darling, 2001). Early gaze stabilisation before
landing probably provides a time margin in which to
use the visual information and control landing.
Accordingly, to reveal how gymnasts look at
locations quickly would be an interesting research
project. From this point of view, it would be
important to note that there is a relationship between
initiation time of gaze stabilisation and head-on-trunk
angle. For achieving early gaze stabilisation, it may
be necessary to rotate the head ahead of the trunk.
Interestingly, the gymnasts who started to stabilise
their gazes earliest before landing rotated their bodies
more. A lack of rotation leads to deductions in
gymnastics (Fédération Internationale de
Gymnastique, 2013). Therefore gymnasts who can
look at locations quicker before landing might have
an advantage in completing rotation, as well as
gaining enough time to use visual information.
Table 1: Correlation coefficients between gaze parameters
and rotational angle in gymnasts.
Variable
Initiation of gaze
stabilization
Landing
Head Trunk
Head-on-
trunk
Head Trunk
Gaze
stabilisation
-.73*
-.92** .77** .75* .81**
Note. Gaze stabilisation = Initiation of gaze stabilisation
**: p < .01. *: p < .05.
-50
50
150
250
350
-50
50
150
250
350
-50
50
150
250
350
0.25 s
-50
50
Takeoff Landing
Head
Gaze
Trunk
Head-on-trunk
Initiation of
gaze stabilization
Angle of rotation (deg)
Relationship between Initiation of Gaze Stabilisation and Angle of Head and Trunk Movement during a Jump with Full Turn
167
We found that initiation time of gaze stabilisation
correlates with magnitude of rotation. However, the
present study cannot identify a way for gymnasts to
look more quickly at locations before landing, the
main factor of the quick look. From this viewpoint, it
would be very useful for a future study to explore how
gymnasts quickly start to stabilise their gaze before
landing.
There is a relationship between gaze behaviour
and body movements during the jump with full turn,
but the relationship between eye-body interactions
and expertise is unclear. Future studies are necessary
to determine the influence of expertise on eye-body
interactions by comparing between experts and
novices (Heinen, 2011).
5 CONCLUSIONS
The aim of this study was to reveal the relationship
between initiation of gaze stabilisation and movement
kinematics before landing. We found various
relationships between initiation of gaze stabilisation
and the jump movement in gymnasts; for example,
positive correlation between gaze stabilisation and
angle of trunk rotation at landing. The results suggest
that gymnasts who can look at locations quicker
before landing might have an advantage in
completing rotation, as well as using visual
information to control landing. For achieving early
gaze stabilisation, it may be necessary to rotate the
head ahead of the trunk. It is hoped that the findings
obtained by the present study will contribute to the
development of new training materials and methods.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This research was supported by JSPS KAKENHI
Grant-in-Aid for Young Scientists (B), Grant Number
26750280.
REFERENCES
Anastasopoulos, D., Ziavra, N., Hollands, M., & Bronstein,
A. (2009). Gaze displacement and inter-segmental
coordination during large whole body voluntary
rotations. Experimental Brain Research, 193(3), 323-
336.
Berthoz, A., & Pozzo, T. (1994). Head and body
coordination during locomotion and complex
movements. In S.P. Swinnen, J. Massion, & H. Herer
(Eds.), Interlimb coordination: Neural, dynamical, and
cognitive constraints (pp. 147-165). San Diego:
Academic Press.
Davlin, C. D., Sands, W. A., & Shultz, B. B. (2004). Do
gymnasts" spot" during a back tuck somersault.
International Sports Journal, 8(2), 72-79.
Fédération Internationale de Gymnastique. (2013). 2013-
2016 Code of Points: Men’s Artistic Gymnastics.
Retrieved from http://www.fig-
gymnastics.com/site/rules/disciplines/art.
Heinen, T., Velentzas, K., & Vinken, P. M. (2012a).
Analyzing gaze behavior in complex (aerial) skills.
International Journal of Sport Science and
Engineering, 6(3), 165-174.
Heinen, T., Velentzas, K., & Vinken, P. M. (2012b).
Functional relationships between gaze behavior and
movement kinematics when performing high bar
dismounts–an exploratory study. Human Movement,
13(3), 218-224.
Hollands, M. A., Ziavra, N. V., & Bronstein, A. M. (2004).
A new paradigm to investigate the roles of head and eye
movements in the coordination of whole-body
movements. Experimental brain research, 154(2), 261-
266.
Hondzinski, J. M., & Darling, W. G. (2001). Aerial
somersault performance under three visual conditions.
Motor Control, 5(3), 281-300.
Laws, K., Sugano, A., & Swope, M. (2002). Pirouettes.
Physics and the art of dance: Understanding movement
(pp. 63-85). New York: Oxford University Press.
Lee, D. N., Young, D. S., & Rewt, D. (1992). How do
somersaulters land on their feet? Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Performance, 18(4), 1195 -1202.
Rezette, D., & Amblard, B. (1985). Orientation versus
motion visual cues to control sensorimotor skills in
some acrobatic leaps. Human Movement Science, 4(4),
297-306.
Sato, Y., Torii, S., Sasaki, M., & Heinen T. (2015). Gaze
Shift Patterns during a Jump with Full Turn in
Gymnasts. Manuscript in preparation.
Sands, W. A. (1991). Spatial orientation while
somersaulting. Technique, 11(1), 16-19.
von Lassberg, C., Beykirch, K. A., Mohler, B. J., &
Bülthoff, H. H. (2014). Intersegmental Eye-Head-Body
Interactions during Complex Whole Body Movements.
PloS one, 9(4), e95450.
icSPORTS 2015 - International Congress on Sport Sciences Research and Technology Support
168