Seeing or Doing?
Pitch Recognition of Batters versus Pitchers: A Preliminary Report
Yin-Hua Chen, Pei-Hong Lee, Yu-Wen Lu and Nai-Shing Yen
Research Center for Mind, Brain and Learning, National Chengchi University
No. 64, Sec. 2, Zhi-Nan Rd., Wen-Shan District, Taipei 11605, Taiwan
Keywords: Baseball, Pitch Recognition, Action Anticipation, Pitcher, Batter.
Abstract: In this study we tackled the question: between the experience of seeing or doing the movement, which one
is more important in understanding the observed movement? We thus asked batters and pitchers, in high and
intermediate skill levels, to identify the type of pitch that was edited in difference lengths. In general, we
found that advanced players showed significant higher accuracy and lower uncertain rate than the
intermediate players, particularly in viewing short pitch sequences. These results reflected the requirement
of fast sports such as baseball, in which players have to make a correct decision quickly rather than staying
uncertain. Moreover, advanced batters showed the tendency of being more accurate than advanced pitchers,
though the difference did not reach statistical significance possibly due to small sample size. In consistency
with the previous studies, all players showed higher accuracy in identifying the strike pitches when they
could see longer sequence of the pitch motion and the baseball trajectory (Paull & Glencross, 1997). In sum,
our results supported the notion that when understanding an observed movement, the perceptuo-motor
experience reacting to it is more important than the actual motor experience of the observed movement.
1 INTRODUCTION
In anticipating the action of the opponent in sports, it
has been shown that the experience plays an
important role (review see Williams, Davids, &
Williams, 1999). That is, due to the accumulated
experience, a skilled athlete knows where to view in
the opponent and then makes the best use of the
information extracted to act or react to the opponent
(e.g., Farrow & Abernethy, 2003; Aglioti et al.,
2008). For example, advanced baseball batters pay
close attention to the pitcher’s motion particularly in
the pitcher’s shoulder, elbow and wrist, and then
switch the focus to the ball trajectory with fewer
fixations than the intermediate batters for making the
batting decision (e.g., Hubbard & Seng, 1954; Shank
& Haywood, 1987; Takeuchi & Inomata, 2009).
In a recent study, it was investigated the ability
to predict the fate of actual or fake soccer penalty
kicks between goalkeepers, kickers and novices
(Tomeo et al., 2012). Goalkeepers showed higher
accuracy for fake actions as compared to kickers and
novices. Kickers were even more confused by the
fake actions than goalkeepers and novices. The
authors concluded that goalkeepers could
outperform kickers and novices due to their visual
rather than motor expertise. However, we thought
that goalkeepers should be considered as “visuo-
motor” experts since they are trained to “perceive
and react” to the penalty kicks. Kickers, instead,
don’t have to intercept the penalty kick even though
they are capable of doing a fool action.
In fact, baseball and football can be considered
very special sports because the players have two
very distinctive roles. Take baseball player for
example: the pitcher is responsible for throwing the
pitch and the batter has to bat and run. The two roles
of players have developed very specialized
perceptual and motor expertise depending on the
task required in the match. Thus, we would like to
investigate whether the pitcher (who possesses the
expertise of performing the pitch motion) or the
batter (who possesses the perceptuo-motor expertise
of intercepting the pitch) could better recognize
whether the pitch is a strike or a ball. We thus asked
elite pitchers and batters to identify whether a pitch
is a strike or a ball and compared their performance
with intermediate players. The pitch sequence was
edited in different lengths to see whether different
amount of the information of the baseball trajectory
could differently influence the pitch identification
17
Chen Y., Lee P., Lu Y. and Yen N..
Seeing or Doing? - Pitch Recognition of Batters versus Pitchers: A Preliminary Report.
DOI: 10.5220/0005144700170024
In Proceedings of the 2nd International Congress on Sports Sciences Research and Technology Support (icSPORTS-2014), pages 17-24
ISBN: 978-989-758-057-4
Copyright
c
2014 SCITEPRESS (Science and Technology Publications, Lda.)
for pitchers and batters with high and low expertise
levels.
We expected that advanced players, both
pitchers and batters, would be more accurate than
intermediate players due to their much more
experience in pitch identification in general.
Furthermore, batters would be more accurate than
pitchers, particularly in advanced group, because
they were the so-called perceptuo-motor experts in
this task in the match.
2 METHODS
2.1 Participants
We recruited 9 high-level pitchers (hereafter HP;
mean age=21; training years=12.52; hours per
week=16.19
) and 18 high-level batters (hereafter
HB; mean age=20; training years=10.22; hours per
week=23.94) from highly ranked Taiwanese
university baseball team in this study. Most of them
had the experience of participating in international
competition. Moreover, a group of intermediate
pitchers (n=7; hereafter IP; mean age=23; training
years=5.43; hours per week=4.14) and intermediate
batters (n=12; hereafter IB; mean age=24; training
years=5.79; hours per week=6.63) were recruited as
control groups. All participants were right-handed
males, and with the height about 180 cm to have
similar strike zone. This study was approved by the
Research ethics committee of National Taiwan
University and was in accordance with the
Declaration of Helsinki; participants gave written
informed consent.
2.2 Stimuli
The stimulus sequences were colour video clips
(wmv format) of baseball pitches of 2 skilled
pitchers. The 2 skilled pitchers were asked to throw
four-seam fastballs to the strike zone of a 180-cm
right-handed batter from the pitcher’s mound toward
the catcher, given a draw situation of full count (2
strikes & 3 balls), 2 out, and full base at the last
inning. The video sequences were taken from the
right-handed batter’s perspective using video camera
(SONY HDR-XR150; 30 frames/s; setting see
Figure 1). 9 strikes and 9 balls thrown by each
pitcher were recorded, making a total of 36 (2
pitchers x 2 types of pitch x 9 throws) different
throws. Whether the pitch was a strike or a ball was
judged by a skilled catcher on site. The criterion of
recruiting the 2 skilled pitchers and catcher was the
same as the criterion of recruiting the advanced
skill-level batters. The average speed of the throws
was controlled at around 115 km/hr by a speed gun.
We then edited each video in 12 different lengths,
which include the windup preparation phase and the
pitching phase till the moment of the baseball
released from the pitcher, or 33, 67, 100, 133, 167,
200, 233, 267, 300, 333, and 367 ms after the
baseball released from the pitcher, respectively.
2.3 Task
The task is twofold. Right after viewing the pitch,
participant had to decide whether he would swing
the bat or not (to bat, not to bat, or uncertain) by
pressing the response key 1, 2, or 3 with index,
middle or ring finger. Immediately after this batting
decision, he had to recognize the pitch type (strike,
ball, or uncertain) again by pressing one response
key with its corresponding finger. The response key
(1, 2, or 3) assigned to the answer of batting decision
(to bat, not to bat, or uncertain) were
counterbalanced between participants. The response
key of the answer of batting decision (to bat, not to
bat, or uncertain) was always combined with the
response key of answer of pitch recognition (strike,
ball, or uncertain) following the nature of batting a
strike and not batting a ball. All of the responses had
to be made in 2.5 s, or the trial would be skipped.
We reminded participants to respond as quickly as
possible, but we emphasised accuracy over speed.
Figure 1: The display of experimental apparatuses: the
blue filled box indicates the position of video camera.
icSPORTS2014-InternationalCongressonSportSciencesResearchandTechnologySupport
18
2.4 Procedure
Before testing, we demonstrated the video sequences
of the 9 strike and 9 ball pitches of each pitcher to
the participant. The video sequences were longer
than the testing stimuli because they were terminated
at the moment of 200 ms before the baseball was
caught by the catcher. This procedure was applied to
let the participants familiar with the strike zone
judged by the catcher and to let participants adapted
to the scene filmed by the video camera. We then
explained the task to the participant and the
participant could practice at least 10 trials to make
sure that the task is fully understood.
In each trial, the participant was presented with a
fixation cross displayed on a white background and
located in the centre of the screen (1024x768, 60Hz)
for 1 s. Next, the video clip of the pitch was played.
When the video clip terminated, participant had to
decide whether to bat or not and to recognize the
pitch type. The inter-stimuli interval (ISI) was 1 s
(See Figure 2). There were 432 (2 pitchers x 2 types
x 9 pitches x 12 video lengths) trials, randomly
divided into 8 runs, to be completed. Between each
runs, participant could have a short break of 3-5
minutes. The entire experiment took approximately
1.5 hr. The experimental protocol was written using
Eprime 2.0. The response and response time of
participants were registered for data analysis.
2.5 Data Analysis
We calculated the correct, incorrect, and uncertain
response in percentage of each participant in each
experimental condition. The data was then entered
into 3 separate repeated-measures mixed-model 3-
way (4 groups x 2 types of pitch x 12 lengths of
video) ANOVAs for the correct, incorrect and
uncertain response, respectively. For all ANOVAs,
group was the between-subject factor, and type of
pitch and length of video were within- subject
factors. The threshold for significance was set at p
< .05. A Bonferroni adjustment was used for
multiple comparisons. SPSS 20.0 was used for
statistical analysis.
3 RESULTS
In Figure 3, we demonstrated the percentage of
“correct”, “incorrect” and “uncertain” response of
pitch identification of four groups for strikes and for
balls, in different lengths of video sequence of the
pitch. The statistics were reported in Table 1.
3.1 Correct Response
The ANOVA detected a significant main effect of
group, with advanced players showing higher
accuracy than intermediate players (mean value of
60%, 62%, 55%, and 55% for HP, HB, IP, and IB,
respectively). Post-hoc analyses indicated that HB
showed significantly higher accuracy than IB. There
was also a significant main effect of pitch type, with
higher accuracy in strikes than in balls. Moreover,
we found a significant pitch type-by-group
interaction, with HB being more accurate than IB
particularly for strikes. And, all groups except for IB
showed higher accuracy in strikes than in balls (see
Figure 4 top panel). The main effect of length of
video was also significant, showing that all players
were more accurate when they could see longer
pitch sequence. Video length-by-group interaction
was also significant, for that HB showed
significantly higher accuracy than intermediate
players (both HP and HB) especially for short videos
(length 1~4; see Figure 4 middle panel). The video
length-by-pitch type interaction was also significant,
with higher accuracy in strikes than in balls
particularly for long videos (length4, and 6~12). The
3-way interaction was not significant.
Figure 2: The procedure of a trial.
SeeingorDoing?-PitchRecognitionofBattersversusPitchers:APreliminaryReport
19
Figure 3: The percentage of “correct”, “incorrect” and “uncertain” response of pitch identification for strikes and for balls
of four groups (HP: high-level pitcher, HB: high-level batter, IP: intermediate pitcher, IB: intermediate batter) after viewing
different lengths of video sequence of the pitch. The 12 different video lengths showed the windup preparation phase and
the pitching phase until the moment of the baseball released from the pitcher, or 33, 67, 100, 133, 167, 200, 233, 267, 300,
333, and 367 ms after the baseball released from the pitcher, respectively.
icSPORTS2014-InternationalCongressonSportSciencesResearchandTechnologySupport
20
Table1: Statistic s of all main effects and interaction effects for correct rate, incorrect rate, and uncertain rate.
Index Effect F value p value Pairwise comparisons
Correct rate
(%)
Group F
(3,42)
= 3.67 p < .05 HB > IB
Pitch type F
(1, 42)
= 38.55 p < .001 Strike > ball
Pitch type-by-group
interaction
F
(3,42)
= 3.29 p < .05
HB > IB for strikes;
pitch type effect for all groups
expect for IB
Video length
F
(11, 462)
=
136.61
p < .001 Long > short
Video length-by-group
interaction
F
(33,462)
= 4.58 p < .001
H > I, in length1&2;
HB > I, in length3 & 4
Video length-by-pitch
type interaction
F
(11, 462)
= 18.54 p < .001
Strike > ball, in long videos
(length 4, and 6~12)
Incorrect rate
(%)
Group F
(
3
,
42
)
= 3.60 p < .05 HP > IP
Pitch type F
(
1
,
42
)
= 42.35 p < .001 Ball > strike
Pitch type-by-group
interaction
F
(3, 42)
= 3.00 p < .05 HB > IB, for balls
Video length F
(
11
,
473
)
= 5.89 p < .001
Video length-by-group
interaction
F
(33, 462)
= 2.77 p < .001
H > I, in length1;
HP > I, in length3;
HP > IP, in length5
Video length-by-pitch
type interaction
F
(11, 462)
= 16.26 p < .001
Ball > strike, in long videos
(length 4~12)
Uncertain rate
(%)
Group F
(
3
,
42
)
= 5.90 p < .005 I > H
Video length F
(
11
,
473
)
= 89.01 p < .001 Short > long
Video length-by-group
interaction
F
(33, 462)
= 5.85 p < .001
I > H, in length1&2;
I > HB, in length3
Video length-by-pitch
type interaction
F
(11, 462)
= 2.65 p < .005
Strike > ball, in length3;
ball > strike, in length11&12
H = high-level pitcher and batter; I = intermediate pitcher and batter; HP = high-level pitcher; HB = high-level batter; IP =
intermediate pitcher; IB = intermediate batter.
3.2 Incorrect Response
The ANOVA detected a significant main effect of
type of pitch, in which balls were identified with
more mistakes than strikes. The main effect of group
was also significant, with HP showing higher
inaccuracy than IP (mean value of 36%, 32%, 28%,
and 29% for HP, HB, IP, and IB, respectively). A
significant pitch type-by-group interaction was
detected. Post-hoc analyses indicated that HB
showed higher inaccuracy than IB particularly for
balls. And, all groups except for IB had higher
inaccuracy for balls than for strikes (see Figure 5 top
panel). The main effect of length of video was also
significant. Moreover, video length-by-group
interaction was also significant, with advanced
players selectively showing higher rate than the
intermediate players in length 1, 3, and 5 (see Figure
5 middle panel). The interaction between the main
effect of pitch type and video length was also
significant. This interaction was due that the
inaccuracy for the strikes decreased as the video
became longer, while the tendency was opposite for
the balls (see Figure 5 bottom panel). The 3-way
interaction was not significant.
3.3 Uncertain Response
We found a significant main effect of group was also
significant, with advanced players, both pitchers and
batters, showing lower uncertain rate than the
intermediate players (mean value of 5%, 7%, 17%,
and 16% for HP, HB, IP, and IB, respectively). The
main effect of length of video was also found
significant, with higher rate for the short videos. The
interaction between the main effect of type of pitch
and the length of video was also found significant.
This interaction was due to a higher rate for strikes
than balls only in length 3 (see Figure 6 bottom
panel). Video length-by-group interaction was also
SeeingorDoing?-PitchRecognitionofBattersversusPitchers:APreliminaryReport
21
significant, for advanced players, no matter pitchers
or batters, showed lower rate than intermediate
players especially for short videos (length 1~3; see
Figure 6 bottom panel). The other two-way or three-
way interactions were not found significant, and
their p values were far from significant level.
Figure 4: Four groups’ average correct response for two
types of pitch (top panel) and for viewing different lengths
of pitch sequence (middle panel); all players average
correct response for strikes and for balls in different video
lengths (the bottom panel). *p < .05. Error bars indicate
standard errors.
Figure 5: Four groups’ average incorrect response for two
types of pitch (top panel) and for viewing different lengths
of pitch sequence (middle panel); and all players average
incorrect response for strikes and for balls in different
video lengths (the bottom panel). *p < .05. Error bars
indicate standard errors.
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
icSPORTS2014-InternationalCongressonSportSciencesResearchandTechnologySupport
22
Figure 6: Four groups’ average uncertain response after
viewing different lengths of pitch sequence (top panel);
and all players average uncertain response for strikes and
for balls in different video lengths (the bottom panel). *p
< .05. Error bars indicate standard errors.
4 DISCUSSION
In this study we tackled the question: in terms of
understanding an observed movement, is the
perceptuo-motor experience reacting to it more
important than the actual motor experience of the
observed movement? Baseball pitchers and players
could be considered as the best candidates to study
this topic since pitchers are trained to throw the pitch,
and batters are trained to intercept it. When they are
required to identify the type of the pitch, who can
show the higher accuracy? We recruited pitchers and
batters, with advanced and intermediate skill levels,
to study this topic.
We found that advanced batters showed the
highest accuracy among the four groups, particularly
when only very short pitching sequence was
presented to them for pitch identification. They were
significantly more accurate than the intermediate
players (both pitchers and batters). Advanced
pitchers were slightly less accurate than the
advanced batters (even though without reaching
statistical significance), and intermediate players
were the worst. Between intermediate pitchers and
batters, there was no significant difference. This
result was consistent with previous finding that
video-motor experts (goalkeepers) were more
accurate than the motor experts (kickers) (Tomeo et
al., 2012) and high level players were more accurate
and faster in their response than intermediate players
(Williams et al., 1999). However, the lack of
significant difference between the 18 advanced
batters and the 9 advanced pitchers could be due to
the unbalanced sample size. We expect to find a
statistical difference when we will recruit more
advanced pitchers in the future. As the indifference
between intermediate pitchers and intermediate
batters, we thought it is reasonable because they
have not developed such great difference since their
training experience was not so different as compared
to advanced players.
In addition, in consistency with the previous
studies, we found that our players generally showed
higher accuracy in identifying the strike pitches
when they could see longer sequence of the pitch
motion and the baseball’s trajectory (Paull &
Glencross, 1997). However, no such difference was
found for ball pitches. For explaining this result, we
would like to note that the final position where the
balls passed were not always very far from the
striking zone (in the perspective of the catcher). For
several balls, the difference to the striking zone was
only the size of half or one baseball. In this case,
these balls were easily to be identified as the strikes,
especially because we set experiment situation as in
a full count (2 strikes & 3 balls), 2 out, and full base
at the last inning.
Furthermore, intermediate players showed the
higher uncertain rate than the advanced players
particularly for short videos. This finding was of
importance because it revealed the requirement of
fast sports such as baseball, in which players have to
make a quick decision rather than staying uncertain.
Last, we found that advanced pitchers showed the
higher inaccuracy rate than the intermediate players.
However, we would not take it as they were more
erroneous than the intermediate players. Instead, we
interpreted it as the result of identifying a pitch as a
strike or a ball, rather than giving an uncertain
response.
*
SeeingorDoing?-PitchRecognitionofBattersversusPitchers:APreliminaryReport
23
In sum, at this moment our results suggested that
advanced players were generally more accurate than
intermediate players. Moreover, there seemed to the
difference between pitchers and batters, particularly
in high levels. Whether the difference is significant
or not has to be confirmed by recruiting more
pitchers and comparing their performance with
batters. This difference could be important because
it supports the notion that athletes (batters) can
better perform the task that they train better than the
athletes (pitchers) that acquire the actual motor
experience in doing the movement (Newell, 1986).
A further analysis of the players’ response time will
be done to better understand their ability in pitch
identification.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We would like to thank coach Shih-Kuei Huang
from Chinese Culture University, coach Wen-Nan
Liao from University of Taipei, coach Jung-Tang
Kung from National Taiwan Sports University, and
coach Po-Hsiu Lin from National Taiwan Normal
University for their help in recruiting baseball
players and their useful insights in experiment setup
and discussion.
REFERENCES
Aglioti, S. M., Cesari, P., Romani, M., & Urgesi, C., 2008.
Action anticipation and motor resonance in elite
basketball players. Nature Neuroscience, 11, pp.
1109–1116.
Newell, K. M., 1986. Constraints on the development of
coordination. In Wade M, Whiting HTA (editors):
Motor Development in Children: Aspects of
Coordination and Control (pp.341-360). Dordrecht,
Germany: Martinus Nijhoff.
Farrow, D. & Abernethy, B., 2003. Implicit perceptual
learning and the significance of chance comparisons:
A response to Jackson. Journal of Sports Sciences, 21,
pp. 511-513.
Farrow, D., & Abernethy, B., 2003. Do expertise and the
degree of perception-action coupling affect natural
anticipatory performance? Perception, 32, pp. 1127-
1139.
Paull, G., & Glencross, D., 1997. Expert perception and
decision-making in baseball. International Journal of
Sport Psychology, 28, pp. 35-56.
Takeuchi, T., & Inomata, K., 2009. Visual search
strategies and decision making in baseball batting.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 108 (3), pp. 971–980.
Tomeo, E., Cesari, P., Aglioti S.M., & Urgesi, C., 2012.
Fooling the Kickers but not the Goalkeepers:
Behavioral and Neurophysiological Correlates of Fake
Action Detection in Soccer. Cerebral Cortex Advance
Access .
Williams, A. M., Davids, K., & Williams, J. G., 1999.
Visual perception and action in sport. London: E. & F.
N. Spon.
icSPORTS2014-InternationalCongressonSportSciencesResearchandTechnologySupport
24