AUTOMATIC SHAKE TO ENHANCE
FRASER-WILCOX ILLUSIONS
Kazuhisa Yanaka, Ryuto Mitsuhashi and Teluhiko Hilano
Kanagawa Institute of Technology, 1030 Shimo-ogino, Atsugi-shi, Kanagawa-ken, 243–0292, Japan
Keywords: Optical illusion, Fraser-wilcox illusion, Illusory motion, Peripheral drift illusion.
Abstract: The Fraser-Wilcox illusion, which is an optical illusion first found by Fraser and Wilcox in 1979 and later
classified into the peripheral drift illusion that was presented in 1997, is the illusion that a disk drawn on a
still image looks as if it is rotating. Recently, Kitaoka proposed an optimized Fraser-Wilcox illusion type V
in which a stronger illusion can be perceived. This proposal has attracted a great deal of attention, but not
everyone can see the illusion. It is well known that the effect of some existing illusions is reinforced by
shaking the image by hand, and we therefore developed a system in which a still image displayed on the
screen of an ordinary PC can be shaken automatically by using our software. Experimental results
demonstrated that the strength of some types of Fraser-Wilcox illusions can be enhanced considerably by
using the proposed system.
1 INTRODUCTION
Optical illusions are thought to be a kind of
malfunction that occurs when the highly
sophisticated human visual system processes retinal
images. The study of optical illusions is therefore
useful in terms of understanding human cognitive
mechanisms.
Among the various illusions that have been
reported up to now, there is a very interesting one,
usually called the illusory motion or the self-
animated image, in which parts of a still picture look
as if they are moving just by glimpsing it. A typical
example is the Fraser-Wilcox illusion (Fraser and
Wilcox, 1979). In this illusion, disks that consist of
repeated asymmetric patterns are drawn on paper or
displayed on a screen and appear to rotate
spontaneously, even though in actuality the image is
perfectly stationary. The illusion is strong enough
for those who are accustomed to it, but it is so faint
that beginners of illusion can hardly see it.
It is empirically known that the strength of an
optical illusion, not restricted to the Fraser-Wilcox
one, can sometimes be reinforced when the figure is
shaken: for example, by observers repeatedly
moving their glasses up and down. However,
reproducibility cannot be expected with such a
method. We could find no reports on how to shake
or with what kind of illusions this method is
effective. Therefore, we developed a system that
automatically shakes an illusion picture displayed on
a PC screen and then studied this phenomenon
experimentally.
2 FRASER-WILCOX ILLUSION
After the discovery of the Fraser-Wilcox illusion, a
similar illusion was reported by Faubert and Herbert
(1997). They called the illusion the peripheral drift
illusion (PDI) because it is caused by peripheral
vision and the illusion disappears when the pattern is
gazed at directly. Since then, the Fraser-Wilcox
illusion has been thought to be a typical example of
PDI. Many researchers have studied it intensively in
an attempt to reveal the mechanism behind such
illusions. One possibility is that the gradient of the
pattern that is scanned by the fixation movement of
the eye cheats the neurons that detect movement. An
interesting theory has recently been presented that
explains illusionary motion by combining a time
filter and a spatial filter (Fermüller, Ji, Kitaoka,
2010). Although this theory is feasible, further
research would be necessary to develop any final
conclusion.
In contrast, intensive experimental studies have
been done to clarify the conditions in which a
stronger illusion is perceived. A representative
405
Yanaka K., Mitsuhashi R. and Hilano T..
AUTOMATIC SHAKE TO ENHANCE FRASER-WILCOX ILLUSIONS .
DOI: 10.5220/0003321904050408
In Proceedings of the International Conference on Computer Vision Theory and Applications (VISAPP-2011), pages 405-408
ISBN: 978-989-8425-47-8
Copyright
c
2011 SCITEPRESS (Science and Technology Publications, Lda.)
example of such studies is Kitaoka’s work. He not
only created a number of novel, beautiful, and strong
“optimized” Fraser-Wilcox illusions, he also
classified the illusions into six types based on the
underlying rules. Among them, Type V (Kitaoka, A.,
2009) is unique because the effect is very strong
compared to other types. In addition, in Type V
color is indispensable, whereas the other types are
basically monochrome. Kitaoka’s explanation of a
similar illusion is that a longer-wavelength color,
such as red, is perceived by our visual system faster
than a shorter-wavelength color, such as blue
(Kitaoka, A., 2010).
However, there are some people who cannot see
an illusion of this type, perhaps because glimpsing is
not easy for them. Therefore, we developed a system
that can move an image displayed on a PC screen,
thus enabling almost everyone to see the illusion.
3 THE SYSTEM
TO SHAKE IMAGES
We used an ordinary Windows PC to shake illusion
images, as shown in Fig. 1.
Figure 1: Our program running on a PC.
(LCD: Mitsubishi RDT 202 WM 20.1 inch, 1680 × 1050).
Our program was written in C language and was
developed with Microsoft Visual Studio 2008. The
program reads the bmp file of a still image that
causes an optical illusion and then displays the
image on the screen. The image is updated tens of
times per second and the position of the image on
the screen is changed bit by bit by controlling an
interval timer. This allows the shaken images to be
displayed.
As shown in Fig. 2, the orientation of the shake
can be designated as horizontal, vertical, diagonal
(top left to bottom right and top right to bottom left),
right rotation, and left rotation.
Figure 2: Orientation of the shake.
As shown in Fig. 3, there are two patterns of
movement: sine wave and triangular wave. The
cycle time of the wave can be varied from 10 to
2000 ms. The height of wave movement can be set
from 0 to 100 pixels.
Figure 3: The case of vertical shake.
4 EXPERIMENTS
In the experiments, we primarily used a basic Fraser-
Wilcox illusion (Fig. 4) and an optimized Fraser-
Wilcox illusion Type V (Fig. 5). The synthesis of a
pattern like the one in Fig. 4 is relatively
straightforward because it consists of simple
gradient patterns. On the other hand, a pattern like
the one in Fig. 5 is rather complex because the
variation of the color plays important roles.
Although the outline of the brightness variation
of the figure that causes this type of illusion has
been described online (Kitaoka, A., 2010), we could
not find detailed information on the variation of the
color anywhere. Therefore, the test patterns used in
our experiment were created from scratch through
trial and error by referring to published papers and
Web pages.
Horizontal Vertical Diagonal
(top left to
bottom right)
Diagonal
(top right to
bottom left)
Right
rotation
Left
rotation
VISAPP 2011 - International Conference on Computer Vision Theory and Applications
406
Figure 4: Basic Fraser-Wilcox illusion.
Figure 5: Optimized Fraser-Wilcox illusion type V.
One of the circular patterns included in Fig. 5
consists of two or more rings that share a center. If a
ring is taken out, as shown in Fig. 6, it has
periodicity along the angle θ. When one cycle is
taken out, each brightness value of the RGB
components is a function of θ because the distance
from the center does not affect the value.
A very interesting question is what kind of
functions should be used in order to obtain the
illusion. We concluded that the green component
should be at least a constant, and its desirable value
is zero. As for the red and blue components, various
functions of θ are possible. For example, sine waves
of the same frequency but different phases, one for
the red component and the other for the blue, are
acceptable, although the illusion is not very strong.
On the other hand, our experiment showed that a
relatively strong illusion can be obtained when the
functions shown in Fig. 7 are used. Please note that
the vertical axis of Fig. 7 has been normalized by the
full brightness and the horizontal axis has been
normalized by the single cycle.
Experimental results showed that the Type V
caused a stronger illusion to be felt. The results also
suggest that an appropriate viewing angle is
necessary for perceiving a strong illusion. In other
words, the viewing angle greatly influences the
effect of the enhancement. For example the rotation
could be seen when the distance between the eyes
and the display was about 30 cm. However, no
rotation could be seen when the distance was about 2
m.
Figure 6: A ring extracted from Fig. 5.
Figure 7: An example of variation of RGB values, each of
which has the function of θ.
When the wave height is not large to some
degree, the illusion is not caused. When the
frequency of the vibration is high, the illusion grows.
However, when the pulse height is small, the illusion
is not generated. The ideal wave height varies with
individuals, but overall it is about 50 pixels. When
the pulse height grows, it becomes difficult to
determine whether the illusion is caused or not.
A supplementary experiment was conducted for
comparison. In this experiment, a hard copy of the
illusion was printed with an ordinary inkjet printer
(Canon MP960) and then hung from a clipboard
with a weight and a spring, as shown in Fig. 8. It
was then shaken by hand.
We confirmed that the optical illusion could be
felt even with this simple system. This means that
the enhancement effect of illusion described in this
paper is not limited to a PC screen. However, the
strength of the illusion with the hand shake system
was weaker than the one with the automatic shake
0.0 0.2
0.4
0.6 0.8
1.0
0.0
1.0
Normalized θ
0.5
Blue component
0.75
One cycle
Green component is
always zero.
Red component
Red Magenta Purple Red
Normalized
brightness
AUTOMATIC SHAKE TO ENHANCE FRASER-WILCOX ILLUSIONS
407
system. The reason for this is currently being
investigated. Presumed causes are a too low
frequency of the shake or the change of color.
Figure 8: Manual shake system with hard copy.
5 CONCLUSIONS
We developed a new technique for enhancing
illusory motion and were able to create a very strong
illusion by shaking a picture of the illusion that was
displayed on a PC screen by a programmed control.
A particularly strong illusion was observed in
Kitaoka's optimized Fraser-Wilcox illusion Type V.
In contrast, in the famous rotating snakes illusion
(Kitaoka, A., 2003), the enforcing effect of the shake
was barely perceived. Among the six “optimized”
Fraser-Wilcox illusions classified by Kitaoka, only
the type V showed considerable enhancement. This
result seems to suggest the possibility that the Type
V is fundamentally different from the others.
Because the Fraser-Wilcox optical illusion is
slight, experimentation has been rather difficult up
to now unless special devices are used. However, the
strength of the optical illusion can be boosted by
introducing the “automatic shake” technique
proposed in this paper, thus enabling easier
experiments. Therefore, we expect this technique to
significantly contribute to the advancement of
illusory motion research.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The authors would like to thank Dr. Masahiro
Suzuki of the Kanagawa Institute of Technology for
his valuable comments.
REFERENCES
Fraser, A. and Wilcox, K. J. (1979). Perception of illusory
movement, In Nature 281, pp. 565–566.
Faubert, J. and Herbert, A. M. (1997). The peripheral drift
illusion: A motion illusion in the visual periphery,
Perception, Volume 28, pp.108–121.
Fermüller, C., Ji, H., and Kitaoka, A. (2010). Illusory
motion due to causal time filtering. Vision Research,
Volume 50, Issue 3, pp. 315–329.
Kitaoka, A. and Ashida, H. (2003). Phenomenal
characteristics of the peripheral drift illusion, VISION,
15, pp. 261–262.
Kitaoka, A. (2003). Rotating snakes. http://www.psy.
ritsumei.ac.jp/~akitaoka/rotsnakee.html.
Kitaoka, A. (2009). Optimized Fraser-Wilcox Illusion
Type V, http://www.psy.ritsumei.ac.jp/~akitaoka/
OFWtypeV.html (in Japanese).
Kitaoka, A. (2010). A brief classification of colour
illusions, Colour: Design & Creativity, Volume 5, No.
3, pp. 1–9.
Murakami, I., Kitaoka, A., and Ashida, H. (2006). A
positive correlation between fixation instability and
the strength of illusory motion in a static display,
Vision Research, Volume 46, Issue 15, July 2006, pp.
2421–2431.
VISAPP 2011 - International Conference on Computer Vision Theory and Applications
408