The Correlation between Personality Trait and Social Curiosity
Rani Agias Fitri
1
, Lounafarsha Wielyanida
1
1
Psychology Department, Faculty of Humanities, Bina Nusantara University, Jakarta, Indonesia 11480
Keyword: Social curiosity, personality traits, big five
Abstract: Social curiosity is a form of curiosity that has significant roles in establishing and maintaining human
relationship. Curiosity is frequently related with personal trait. The previous study about variety of
curiosities finds correlation with openness to experience. However, there is correlation with extraversion in
social curiosity. The previous study only correlates with three traits of Big Five personality trait, which are
Extraversion, Neuroticism, and Agreeableness. Therefore, previous study does not discover the correlation
of other traits than those three. This study aims to discover the correlation between five traits of Big Five
personality trait. This study spreads two questionnaires to collect the research data, which are Social
Curiosity Scale (SCS) and Big Five Inventory (BFI) 44. There is correlation between social curiosity with
extraversion, conscientiousness, and openness to experience in 136 participants in this research.
1 INTRODUCTION
Social curiosity is desire to achieve new information
about how other people behave, think, and feel
which is representing basic motivational behavior
system (Hartung and Renner, 2011; Hartung and
Renner, 2013; Renner, 2006). Social curiosity has
significant role in social interaction and human
relationship (Han, Li, Warren, Feng, Litman and Li,
2013). Interpersonal relation is the basic and theme
of human life, that is most human behavior
happened in the context of relationship with other
people (Kasdhan, Stiksma, Disabato, McKnight,
Bekier, Kaji and Lazarus, 2013). Therefore,
information about other people is indispensable in
daily life.
Social curiosity is one form one form of curiosity
that originated from internal motivation (Peterson
and Seligman, 2004). Curiosity happened because
human has congenital desire to handle uncertainty
(Hsee and Ruan, 2016). This makes curiosity
considered as powerful basic human motive
(Kasdhan, Stiksma, Disabato, McKnight, Bekier,
Kaji and Lazarus, 2013). As powerful basic human
motive, curiosity is frequently related with
personality trait. Personality trait is being assumed
as mindset and acquired behaviour, which is only
found in organism with sophisticated cognitive
system (McCrae, Costa, Ostendorf, Angleitner,
Hrebickova, Avia, Sanz, Sanchez-Bernardos, Kusdil,
Woodfield, Saunders and Smith, 2000). Trait has
significant role to predict and understand the variety
of human behaviours (Fleeson and Gallagher, 2009;
Paunonen, 2003; Paunonen and Ashton, 2001). It
also can be used to understand the variety of
exploration behaviours in satisfying general
curiosity as well as social curiosity.
Generally, traits that are related with curiosity
are openness to experience, neuroticism, and
conscientiousness (Hunter, Abraham, Hunter,
Golberg and Eastwood, 2016; Kashdan, Afram,
Brown, Birbeck and Drvoshanov, 2011; Kashdan,
Rose and Fincham, 2004; Kashdan and Steger,
2007). Among the traits that are correlated with
curiosity, neuroticism correlated negatively with it
(Kashdan, Rose and Fincham, 2004; Kashdan and
Steger, 2007). The type of curiosity that is correlated
with personality trait shows different results such as
the correlation between entrepreneurial curiosity
with openness to experience (Jeraj, Maric,
Todorovic, Cudanov and Komazee, 2015), the
correlation between epistemic curiosity with
extraversion (Hunter, Abraham, Hunter, Golberg
and Eastwood, 2016) the weak correlation between
interpersonal curiosity with personality trait, and
neuroticism as the highest correlation with
interpersonal curiosity among other traits (Litman,
Robinson and Demetre, 2016).
Social curiosity has correlation with
extraversion, whereas the sub scales of general
92
Fitri, R. and Wielyanida, L.
The Correlation between Personality Trait and Social Curiosity.
DOI: 10.5220/0010003100002917
In Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Social Sciences, Laws, Arts and Humanities (BINUS-JIC 2018), pages 92-98
ISBN: 978-989-758-515-9
Copyright
c
2022 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
social curiosity correlated with extraversion and the
sub scales of covert social curiosity correlated with
neuroticism (Paunonen, 2003). However, the result
of correlation between social curiosity with personal
trait cannot be determined because the previous
study is only correlating social curiosity with three
out of five traits in Big Five personality trait, which
are extraversion, neuroticism, and agreeableness.
Based on the result of this study, it cannot be as
certain that social curiosity is only correlated with
extraversion. Social curiosity is potentially having
correlation with openness to experience and
conscientiousness. The correlation between curiosity
with openness to experience is highly possible
because curiosity is attached to openness to
experience (Kasdhan, Stiksma, Disabato, McKnight,
Bekier, Kaji and Lazarus, 2013). Openness to
experience characterizes people who are willing –
often excited to deal with any kind of ideas,
feelings, and activities; preference towards variety
and novelty (McCrae and Greenberg, 2014; Woo,
Chernyshenko, Longley, Zhang, Chiu and Stark,
2014). Openness to experience has a role in
friendship and social relationship by searching
similarity with others. The problem between
interpersonal relationship is related with lack of
openness to experience. Closed individuals are hard
to understand and adapt towards others perspective,
which is representing no flexibility (McCrae, 1996).
Openness indicates the readiness of people to adapt
their attitude and behaviour when dealing with new
idea and situation (Onraet, Van Hiel, Roets and
Cornelis, 2011).
Conscientiousness reflects mindset, feeling, and
behaviour that relatively settled, automatically
differs one person to another, and appears through a
situation that will encourage the trait (Paunonen and
Ashton, 2001). Conscientiousness has proactive and
inhibitive aspects. Proactive aspect related with the
behaviour of successful work, whereas inhibition
related with self-control and restriction (Costa and
McCrae, 1998; Costa, McCrae and Dye 1991). Even
though conscientiousness is frequently related with
academic achievement and work performance, it can
be also associated with social curiosity, because of
the significant role in developing and maintaining
successful relationship (Baker and McNully, 2014).
Conscientiousness is also associated with better
interpersonal relationship and great motivation to
solve the relationship problem (Jensen-Campbell,
Knack and Gomez, 2010).
Openness to experience and conscientiousness
have correlation potential with social curiosity. To
prove what trait that is correlated with social
curiosity, it is necessary to do a study to see the
correlation of social curiosity with five traits of Big
Five personality trait. This topic will be the focus of
this study.
2 METHOD
2.1 Participant
Participant in this study consist of 136 participants
in emerging adulthood group. This age group is
being used in this research because according to the
previous study, they show high level of social
curiosity than the older group (Renner, 2006). There
are 40 male participants (29.4%) and 96 female
participants (70,6%). The age of participants ranged
between 18-25 years old (M:20.31, SD: 1.93).
2.2 Procedures
This study uses quantitative method and spreads
research questionnaire to gather the data. Research
questionnaire spreads by online to the students of
Bina Nusantara University and for general
population between 18 until 25 years old, that
matched with emerging adulthood age group
(Arnett, 2007).
Therefore, there are only participants who are
willing to fill the research questionnaire. Research
questionnaire consist of two parts, which are firstly,
the profile of participants (age and gender) and the
second part contains two research questionnaires
that had to be filled
2.3 Measurements
2.3.1 Social Curiosity
Social Curiosity Scale (SCS; α:.764) that is
developed by Renner (Renner, 2006) to measure
social curiosity being used in this study. SCS
consists of two sub scales, which are General Social
Curiosity (SCS-G; α: .725) and Covert Social
Curiosity (SCS-C; α: .805) sub scale that contains 5
questions of each. SCS-G explains curiosity in
others’ habits, feeling, and thought. For example,
I’m interested in people’. SCS-C consists items
such as overheard others’ conversation or observing
others quietly. For example, I like to look into
others’ lit of windows”. SCS uses Likert scale from
1 (strongly disagree) 4 (strongly agree) (Hartung
and Renner, 2013).
The Correlation between Personality Trait and Social Curiosity
93
2.3.2 Personality Trait
Big Five Inventory (BFI) 44 that is developed by
John and Srivastava (John and Srivastava, 1999)
being used to measure the personality trait. BFI 44
consist of 44 items with Likert scale from 1
(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Example of
its items are is talkative’, is helpful and unselfish
with others’, does a thorough job’, is depressed,
blue’, is original, comes up with new ideas’.
Reliability of BFI 44: α Openness: .657, α
Conscientiousness: .746, α Extraversion: .840, α
Agreeableness: .702, and α Neuroticism: .732.
3 RESULT AND DISCUSSIONS
There is correlation between social curiosity with
extraversion in this study (p < .05, r: .215),
conscientiousness (p < .05, r: .209), and openness to
experience (p < .05; r: .207). General sub scales of
social curiosity correlated with agreeableness (p <
.05, r: .369), extraversion (p < .05, r: .322),
conscientiousness (p < .05, r: .289), and openness to
experience (p < .05, r: .001). Covert sub scales of
social curiosity correlated with conscientiousness (p
< .05, r: -.182) and neuroticism (p < .05, r: .225).
As predicted on research hypothesis, social
curiosity is not only correlated with extraversion, but
also with openness to experience and
conscientiousness. Similar with the result of
previous study (Renner, 2006), social curiosity
correlated with extraversion. Extraverts are being
characterized as assertive, talkative, and motivated
to involve with social contact (Tov, Nai and Lee,
2014). People with higher level of extraversion tend
to interact frequently and spend a lot of time with
others than people with lower level of extraversion
(Nezlek, Schutz, Schroder-Abe and Smith, 2011).
Extraversion has relation with positive affect
(Smillie, DeYoung and Hall, 2014). Both relations
happened because extraverts are assertive toward
others (Smillie, Wilt, Kabbani, Garratt and Revelle,
2015). Assertiveness as the characterization of
extraverts can push them to ask all information
about someone who is interacting with them. As we
know that assertive people put interest to know
others’ mind, feeling and behaviour, that can provide
positive affect. Curiosity itself have correlation with
positive affect (Quevedo and Abella, 2011).
In this research, correlation between extraversion
and social curiosity have low correlation. This
correlation happens because extraverts are assertive
and full of enthusiasm (Gottlieb, Oudeyer, Lopes
and Baranes, 2013), therefore they don’t use covert
manners, such as overhearing or observing others to
satisfy their social curiosity. On the contrary,
extraverts will participate actively in their
environment. This participation can produce positive
affect on extraverts [44], therefore they enjoy
interacting with other people directly rather than
using other ways. This can be seen by the correlation
between sub scale SCS-G with extraversion and no
correlation between sub scales SCS-C with
extraversion.
Openness to experience correlated with social
curiosity can happened because having this trait can
drive individuals to learn about other people
including their interacting partner. Individuals with
openness to experience will spend a lot of time by
observing their interaction partner and less talk
about themselves. Individuals that open about
curiosity and attentive to their surroundings, and in
the process to know new people, curiosity will make
them to give more attention toward their interacting
partner, as an effort to observe about their own
partner (McCrae and Sutin, 2009). Therefore,
individuals with openness to experience need to
improve their social curiosity. It is needed to help
people learn about how other people in a community
think, behave, and notice what will they do
afterward (Hartung and Renner, 2011).
Table 1: Personality trait and social curiosity.
Even though there is correlation between
openness to experience and social curiosity, the
correlation between them is being considered as low.
This happens because openness to experience is
more related with cognitive and creativity factors,
BINUS-JIC 2018 - BINUS Joint International Conference
94
such as intellectual curiosity, intellectual interest,
perceived intelligence, imagination, creativity,
artistic and aesthetic interests, emotional and fantasy
richness, and unconventional person (Onraet, Van
Hiel, Roets and Cornelis, 2011). Furthermore, from
6 facets of openness to experience (fantasy,
aesthetics, feelings, actions, ideas, and values) only
one facet is associated with social curiosity, which is
values. Individuals with facet openness to values
tend to be always questioning about social, political,
or religion values (Hartung and Renner, 2011).
Association between openness to experience and
social curiosity only related with people’s effort in
learning about social values and norms through other
people.
Sub scales of SCS-G correlates with
extraversion, openness to experience,
conscientiousness, and agreeableness in this study.
The explanation of relation between sub scales SCS-
G with extraversion and openness to experience is
similar with the correlation of social curiosity to
those three traits. It happened because SCS-G is
approximately had similar meaning with social
curiosity, which are curiosity to other people’s
habits, feeling, and thought (Hartung and Renner,
2013). Agreeableness is correlated with SCS-G
because it is indispensable trait in interpersonal
relationship compared with the other four traits in
Big Five personality trait. Agreeableness
characterized as cooperative, friendly, considerate,
and helpful (Jensen-Campbell, Knack and Gomez,
2010). Agreeableness people are pro social, which is
more likely to keep social harmony (Brutus and
Witenberg, 2012). Compassion facet of
agreeableness makes it related to the commitment of
long-term relationship (Kern, Duckworth, Uruzua,
Loeber and Stouthamer-Loeber, 2013). People with
agreeableness discover the needs of others to show
their affection, being able to work together, as an
effort to show friendly attitude, as well as
encouragement to help others. This makes people
with agreeableness are always motivated to gain
information about others.
SCS-G also correlated with conscientiousness.
Conscientiousness will shape the quality of long-
term relationship. It is possible because people with
conscientiousness are capable to manage conflict in
a relationship. People with conscientiousness tend to
show controlled and organized behaviour, and as a
result they rarely being criticized by others
(Paunonen and Ashton, 2001). To show controlled
and organized behaviour, people with
conscientiousness need have information about
knowledge and rules on how to behave (Hartung and
Renner, 2011).
Similar with previous study (Renner, 2006), this
study finds correlation between neuroticism with sub
scales of SCS-C. Neuroticism is personality trait that
characterized with chronic negative affect, involving
sadness, worry, petulant, and self-conscientiousness,
which are easily triggered and difficult to control
(Ireland, Hepler, Li, and Albarracin, 2014). It makes
people with high neuroticism is easy to worry,
depressed, and feeling guilty. They are more self-
conscious and believing that they have experiencing
threatening and stressful event (Kandler, 2012).
Neuroticism itself is related to intolerance of
uncertainty. On the contrary, people with high
intolerance of uncertainty will encounter greater
worry in daily hassles (Chen and Hong, 2010).
Uncertain condition will encourage people to search
information (Gottlieb, Oudeyer, Lopes and Baranes,
2013) and develop curiosity (Jirout and Klahr,
2012). This uncertainty can happen because of no
access or no ways to gain expected information
directly from the source. It makes people with
neuroticism become more curious and increase the
stress. Therefore, they are encouraged to use covert
way to fulfil their social curiosity. They will
purposely overhearing other people’s conversation
or observing others secretly (Hartung and Renner,
2013).
Correlation between sub scales of SCS-C with
neuroticism is being considered as weak. This level
of correlation can happen because other than
correlating with neuroticism, SCS-C also correlated
with conscientiousness. People with neuroticism
tends to act impulsively and have difficulty in
control (Kandler, 2012). But on the hand, the
negative correlation with conscientiousness can
create an effort to control the satisfy of social
curiosity by using covert ways. This makes people
who tries to resolve the uncertainty about other
people to not fully express the satisfaction of the
curiosity about other people by eavesdropping their
conversations or observing their behaviour. People is
still trying to control their actions.
Sub scales of SCS-C is not only correlated with
neuroticism but also conscientiousness, even though
both of it have negative correlation in this study.
Conscientiousness can be defined as tendency to
follow the norms that are given socially to control
the impulse, leads to the aim, to plan, able to
postpone satisfaction, also follow the norms and
rules (Robert, Jackson, Fayard, Edmonds and
Meints, 2009). One of conscientiousness
characterizations is ability to postpone satisfaction.
The Correlation between Personality Trait and Social Curiosity
95
When they are desperately demand some
information, including about others, people with
conscientiousness are not using any way to satisfy
their curiosity. They are not encouraged to do covert
social curiosity, which is explained as behaviour to
observe or eavesdropping other people (Hartung and
Renner, 2013).
Correlation between sub scale of SCS-C with
conscientiousness can be classified as weak. This
also causes weak correlation between social
curiosity with conscientiousness. This correlation
can happen because SCS-C is assumed not related
with the most common domain of conscientiousness,
which is orderliness and industriousness. SCS-C
only related with self-control domain from
conscientiousness. Self-control is one of the qualities
from consciousness that related with interpersonal
relationship (Renner, 2006). Self-control represent
the tendency to control impulsiveness (Kandler,
2012). The impulse that being controlled here is the
impulsiveness to not satisfy their social curiosity
with covert ways.
4 LIMITATIONS
This study has limitation on the amounts of
participants and focus on only one group age, that is
emerging adulthood. It needs more research to
involve more people and age group. It is needed to
test the constancy of relation between social
curiosity with extraversion trait, openness to
experience, and conscientiousness. As well as the
constancy of relation between sub scales of social
curiosity with other traits that have correlation in
this study. Even though this study has weakness, it
also gives benefit to a study about the correlation
between social curiosity with personality trait. This
study is conducted on different setting of culture
with the previous study (Renner, 2006), which is in
collectivist culture. The result of this study can give
different perspective related with the relation
between social curiosity and personality trait that is
not universal.
5 CONCLUSION
The result of this study has similar and different
results from previous studies. The similarity is on
relation between social curiosity and sub scales of
SCS-G with extraversion and sub scales of SCS-C
with neuroticism. The differences are finding
relation between openness to experience and
conscientiousness with social curiosity; relation
between openness to experience, conscientiousness,
and agreeableness with sub scales of SCS-G; and
relation between conscientiousness with sub scales
of SCS-C. Different findings from previous study
induce some implications. First, lot of correlated
traits with social curiosity are showing its
complexity as applied in the setting of this study,
which is in collective culture country. It is different
with previous study (Renner, 2006) that applied on
individualist culture. Second, related with the first
implication, the study about social curiosity in
collective culture becomes very relevant. Third,
further research is necessary to deeply explore about
the form of social curiosity in collectivist culture
REFERENCES
Albrecht, A.G., Dilchert, S., Deller, J. and Paulus, F.M.
(2014) ’Openness in cross cultural work settings: a
multicountry study of expatriates’, Journal of
Personality Assessment, 96(1), pp. 64-75.
Arnett, J.J. (2007) ’Emerging adulthood: what is it, and
what is it good for?’, Child Development Perspectives,
1(2), pp. 68-73.
Baker, L.R. and McNulty, J.K. (2014) ’Self compassion
and relationship maintenance: the moderating roles of
conscientiousness and gender’, Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 100(5), pp. 853-873.
Brutus, N. and Witenberg, R.T. (2012) ’Some personality
predictors of tolerance to human diversity: the roles of
openness, agreeableness, and empathy’, Australian
Psychological Association, vol. 48, pp. 290-298.
Chen, C.Y. and Hong, R.Y. (2010) ’Intolerance of
uncertainty moderates the relation between negative
life events and anxiety’, Personality and Individual
Differences, vol. 49, pp. 49-53.
Costa, P.L., McCrae, R.R. and Dye, D. (1991)Facet
scales for agreeableness and conscientiousness: a
revision of the NEO Personality Inventory’,
Personality and Individual Differences, 12(9), pp.
887-898.
Costa, P.L. and McCrae, R.R. (1998) ‘Six approaches to
the explication of facet-level traits: examples and
conscientiousness’, European Journal of Personality,
vol. 12, pp. 117-134.
Fleeson, W. and Gallagher, P. (2009) ’The implications of
big five standing for the distribution of trait
manifestation in behavior: fifteen experience-sampling
studies and a meta-analysis’, Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 97(6), pp. 1097-111.
Gottlieb, J., Oudeyer, P.Y., Lopes, M. and Baranes, A.
(2013) ’Information seeking, curiosity, and attention:
computational and neural mechanism’, Trends in
Cognitive Science, 17(11), pp. 585-593.
BINUS-JIC 2018 - BINUS Joint International Conference
96
Han, C., Li, P., Warren, C., Feng, T., Litman, J. and Li, H.
(2013) ’Electrophysiological evidence for the
importance of interpersonal curiosity’, Brain
Research, 1500, pp. 45-54.
Hartung, F.M. and Renner, B. (2013) ’Social curiosity and
gossip: Related but different drives of social
functioning’, PLosONE, 8(7), e69996.
Hartung, F.M. and Renner, B. (2011) ’Social curiosity and
interpersonal perception: a judge x trait interaction’,
Personal and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(6), pp.
796-814.
Hsee, C.K. and Ruan, B. (2016) ’The pandora effect: the
power and peril of curiosity’, Association for
Psychological Science, 27(5), pp. 659-666.
Hunter, J.A., Abraham, E.H., Hunter, A.G., Golberg, L.C.
and Eastwood, J.D. (2016) ’Personality and boredom
proneness in the prediction of creativity and curiosity’,
Thinking Skills and Creativity, vol. 22, pp. 48-57.
Ireland, M.E., Hepler, L., Li, H. and Albarracin, D. (2014)
’Neuroticism and attitudes toward action in 19
countries’, Journal of Personality, 83(3), pp. 243-250.
Jensen-Campbell, L.A., Knack, J.M. and Gomez, H.L.
(2010) ’The psychology of nice people’, Social and
Personality Psychology Compass, 4(11), pp. 1042-
1056.
Jeraj, M., Maric, M., Todorovic, I., Cudanov, M. and
Komazee, S. (2015) ’The role of openness and
entrepreneurial curiosity in company’s growth’,
Amfiteatru Economic, 17(38), pp. 371-389.
Jirout, J. and Klahr, D. (2012) ’Children’s scientific
curiosity: in search of an operational definition of an
elusive concept’, Developmental Review, vol. 32, pp.
125-160.
John, O.P. and Srivastava, S. (1999) ’The Big-Five trait
taxonomy: history, measurement, and theoretical
perspectives’ in Pervin, L.A and Joh, O.P. (eds.),
Handbook of personality: Theory and research, vol. 2.
New York: Guilford Press, pp. 102–138.
Kandler, C. (2012) ’Nature and nurture in personality
development: the case of neuroticism and
extraversion’, Current Direction in Psychological
Science, 21(5), pp. 290-296.
Kashdan, T.B., Afram, A., Brown, K.W., Birbeck, M. and
Drvoshanov, M. (2011) ’Curiosity enhances the role of
mindfulness in reducing defensive responses to
existential threat’, Personality and Individual
Differences, vol. 50, pp. 1227-1232.
Kashdan, T.B. and Steger, M.F. (2007)’Curiosity and
pathways to wellbeing and meaning in life: traits,
states, and everyday behavior’, Motivation and
Emotion, 31(3), p159-173.
Kashdan, T.B., Rose, P. and Fincham, F.D. (2004)
’Curiosity and exploration: facilitating positive
subjective experiences and personal growth
opportunities’, Journal of Personality Assessment,
82(3), pp. 291-305.
Kasdhan, T.B., Stiksma, M.C., Disabato, D.J., McKnight,
P.E., Bekier, J., Kaji, J. and Lazarus, R. (2018) ’The
five-dimensional curiosity scale: capturing the
bandwidth of curiosity and identifying four unique
subgroups of curious people’, Journal of Research in
Personality, 73, p130-149.
Kaufman, S.B. (2013)’Openning up opening to
experience: a four-factor model and relations to
creative achievement in the arts and sciences, The
Journal of Creative Behavior, 47(4), p233-255.
Kern, M.L., Duckworth, A.L., Urzua, S.S., Loeber, R. and
Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2013)’Do as you’re told!
Facets of agreeableness and early adult outcomes for
inner city boys’, Journal of Research in Psychology,
vol. 47, pp. 795-799.
Litman, J.A., Robinson, O.C. and Demetre, J.D. (2016)
‘Intrapersonal curiosity: inquisitives about the inner
self’, Journal Self and Identity, 16(2), pp. 1-20.
McCrae, R.R. and Greenberg, D.M. (2014) ’Openness to
Experience’, in Simonton, D.K (ed.) The Wiley
Handbook of Genius. West Sussex: Wiley Blackwell,
pp. 222-243.
McCrae, R.R. and Sutin, A.R. (2009) ’Openness to
experience’, in Leary M.R. and Hoyle, R.H (eds.)
Handbook of individual differences in social behavior.
New York: Guilford Press, pp. 257-273.
McCrae, R.R., Costa, P.T., Ostendorf, F., Angleitner, A.,
Hrebickova, M., Avia, M.D., Sanz, J., Sanchez-
Bernardos, M.L., Kusdil, M.E., Woodfield, R.,
Saunders, P.R. and Smith, P.B. (2000) ’Nature over
nurture: temperament, personality, and life span
development’, Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 78(1), pp. 173-186.
McCrae, R.R. (1996) ’Social consequences of experiential
openness’, Psychological Bulletin, 120(3), pp. 323-
337.
Nezlek, J. B., Schutz, A., Schroder-Abe, M. and Smith,
C.V. (2011) ’A cross cultural study on relationship
between daily social interaction and the five factor
model of personality’, Journal of Personality, 79(4),
pp. 811-840.
Onraet, E., Van Hiel, A., Roets, A. and Cornelis I (2011)
‘The closed mind: ‘experience’ and ‘cognition’ aspects
of openness to experience and need for closure as
psychological bases for right wing attitudes’,
European Journal of Personality, vol. 25, pp. 184-
197.
Paunonen, S.V. and Ashton, M.C. (2001) ’Big five factors
and facets and prediction of behavior’, Journal of
Personality and Individual Differences, 81(3), pp.
524-539.
Paunonen, S.V. (2003) ’Big five factors of personality and
replicated predictions of behavior’, Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 84(2), pp. 411-
424.
Peterson, C. and Seligman, M.E.P. (2004) ’Character
Strengths and Virtues’, A Handbook and
Classification. New York : Oxford University Pers,
pp. 125-141.
Quevedo, R.J.M. and Abella, M.C. (2011) ’Wellbeing and
personality: facet level analyses’, Personality and
Individual Differences, vol. 50, pp. 206-211.
Renner, B. (2006) ’Curiosity about people: The
development of a social curiosity measure in adults’,
The Correlation between Personality Trait and Social Curiosity
97
Journal of Personality Assessment, 87(3), pp. 305-
316.
Roberts, B.W., Jackson, J.J., Fayard, J.V., Edmonds, G.,
and Meints, J. (2009) ’Conscientiousness Handbook of
Individual Differences in Social Behavior, in Leary,
M.R. and Hoyle, R.H. New York: Guilford, pp. 369-
381.
Roberts, B.W., Lejuez, C., Krueger, R.F., Richards, J.M.
and Hill, P.L. (2014)’What is conscientiousness and
how it can be assessed?’, Journal Development
Psychology, 50(5), pp. 1315-1330.
Smillie, L.D., DeYoung, C.G. and Hall, P.J. (2014)
‘Clarifying the relationship between extraversion and
positive affect’, Journal of Personality, 83(5), pp. 564-
574.
Smillie, L.D., Wilt, J., Kabbani, R., Garratt, C. and
Revelle, W. (2015) ’Quality of social experience
explain the relation between extraversion and positive
affect’, Emotion, 15(3), pp. 339-349.
Srivastava, S., Angelo, K.M. and Vallereux, S.R. (2008)
‘Extraversion and positive affect: a day reconstruction
study a person environment transactions’, Journal of
Research in Personality, vol. 42, pp. 1613-1618.
Tov, W., Nai, Z.L. and Lee, H.W. (2014) ’Extraversion
and agreeableness: divergent routes to daily
satisfaction with social relationship, Journal of
Personality, 84(1), pp. 121-134.
Woo, S.E., Chernyshenko, O.S., Longley, A., Zhang, Z.X.,
Chiu, C.Y. and Stark, S.E. (2014) ‘Openness to
experience: its lower level structure, measurement, and
cross-cultural equivalence’, Journal of Personality
Assessment, 96(1), pp. 29-45.
BINUS-JIC 2018 - BINUS Joint International Conference
98